- Dutch
- Frisian
- Saterfrisian
- Afrikaans
-
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological processes
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Word stress
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Monomorphemic words
- Diachronic aspects
- Generalizations on stress placement
- Default penultimate stress
- Lexical stress
- The closed penult restriction
- Final closed syllables
- The diphthong restriction
- Superheavy syllables (SHS)
- The three-syllable window
- Segmental restrictions
- Phonetic correlates
- Stress shifts in loanwords
- Quantity-sensitivity
- Secondary stress
- Vowel reduction in unstressed syllables
- Stress in complex words
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Accent & intonation
- Clitics
- Spelling
- Morphology
- Word formation
- Compounding
- Nominal compounds
- Verbal compounds
- Adjectival compounds
- Affixoids
- Coordinative compounds
- Synthetic compounds
- Reduplicative compounds
- Phrase-based compounds
- Elative compounds
- Exocentric compounds
- Linking elements
- Separable complex verbs (SCVs)
- Gapping of complex words
- Particle verbs
- Copulative compounds
- Derivation
- Numerals
- Derivation: inputs and input restrictions
- The meaning of affixes
- Non-native morphology
- Cohering and non-cohering affixes
- Prefixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixation: person nouns
- Conversion
- Pseudo-participles
- Bound forms
- Nouns
- Nominal prefixes
- Nominal suffixes
- -aal and -eel
- -aar
- -aard
- -aat
- -air
- -aris
- -ast
- Diminutives
- -dom
- -een
- -ees
- -el (nominal)
- -elaar
- -enis
- -er (nominal)
- -erd
- -erik
- -es
- -eur
- -euse
- ge...te
- -heid
- -iaan, -aan
- -ief
- -iek
- -ier
- -ier (French)
- -ière
- -iet
- -igheid
- -ij and allomorphs
- -ijn
- -in
- -ing
- -isme
- -ist
- -iteit
- -ling
- -oir
- -oot
- -rice
- -schap
- -schap (de)
- -schap (het)
- -sel
- -st
- -ster
- -t
- -tal
- -te
- -voud
- Verbs
- Adjectives
- Adverbs
- Univerbation
- Neo-classical word formation
- Construction-dependent morphology
- Morphological productivity
- Compounding
- Inflection
- Inflection and derivation
- Allomorphy
- The interface between phonology and morphology
- Word formation
- Syntax
- Preface and acknowledgements
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of verb phrases I:Argument structure
- 3 Projection of verb phrases II:Verb frame alternations
- Introduction
- 3.1. Main types
- 3.2. Alternations involving the external argument
- 3.3. Alternations of noun phrases and PPs
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.3.1.1. Dative alternation with aan-phrases (recipients)
- 3.3.1.2. Dative alternation with naar-phrases (goals)
- 3.3.1.3. Dative alternation with van-phrases (sources)
- 3.3.1.4. Dative alternation with bij-phrases (possessors)
- 3.3.1.5. Dative alternation with voor-phrases (benefactives)
- 3.3.1.6. Conclusion
- 3.3.1.7. Bibliographical notes
- 3.3.2. Accusative/PP alternations
- 3.3.3. Nominative/PP alternations
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.4. Some apparent cases of verb frame alternation
- 3.5. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of verb phrases IIIa:Selection of clauses/verb phrases
- 5 Projection of verb phrases IIIb:Argument and complementive clauses
- Introduction
- 5.1. Finite argument clauses
- 5.2. Infinitival argument clauses
- 5.3. Complementive clauses
- 6 Projection of verb phrases IIIc:Complements of non-main verbs
- 7 Projection of verb phrases IIId:Verb clusters
- 8 Projection of verb phrases IV: Adverbial modification
- 9 Word order in the clause I:General introduction
- 10 Word order in the clause II:Position of the finite verb (verb-first/second)
- 11 Word order in the clause III:Clause-initial position (wh-movement)
- Introduction
- 11.1. The formation of V1- and V2-clauses
- 11.2. Clause-initial position remains (phonetically) empty
- 11.3. Clause-initial position is filled
- 12 Word order in the clause IV:Postverbal field (extraposition)
- 13 Word order in the clause V: Middle field (scrambling)
- 14 Main-clause external elements
- Nouns and Noun Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of noun phrases I: complementation
- Introduction
- 2.1. General observations
- 2.2. Prepositional and nominal complements
- 2.3. Clausal complements
- 2.4. Bibliographical notes
- 3 Projection of noun phrases II: modification
- Introduction
- 3.1. Restrictive and non-restrictive modifiers
- 3.2. Premodification
- 3.3. Postmodification
- 3.3.1. Adpositional phrases
- 3.3.2. Relative clauses
- 3.3.3. Infinitival clauses
- 3.3.4. A special case: clauses referring to a proposition
- 3.3.5. Adjectival phrases
- 3.3.6. Adverbial postmodification
- 3.4. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of noun phrases III: binominal constructions
- Introduction
- 4.1. Binominal constructions without a preposition
- 4.2. Binominal constructions with a preposition
- 4.3. Bibliographical notes
- 5 Determiners: articles and pronouns
- Introduction
- 5.1. Articles
- 5.2. Pronouns
- 5.3. Bibliographical notes
- 6 Numerals and quantifiers
- 7 Pre-determiners
- Introduction
- 7.1. The universal quantifier al 'all' and its alternants
- 7.2. The pre-determiner heel 'all/whole'
- 7.3. A note on focus particles
- 7.4. Bibliographical notes
- 8 Syntactic uses of noun phrases
- Adjectives and Adjective Phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- 2 Projection of adjective phrases I: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adjective phrases II: Modification
- 4 Projection of adjective phrases III: Comparison
- 5 Attributive use of the adjective phrase
- 6 Predicative use of the adjective phrase
- 7 The partitive genitive construction
- 8 Adverbial use of the adjective phrase
- 9 Participles and infinitives: their adjectival use
- 10 Special constructions
- Adpositions and adpositional phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Introduction
- 1.1. Characterization of the category adposition
- 1.2. A formal classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3. A semantic classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3.1. Spatial adpositions
- 1.3.2. Temporal adpositions
- 1.3.3. Non-spatial/temporal prepositions
- 1.4. Borderline cases
- 1.5. Bibliographical notes
- 2 Projection of adpositional phrases: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adpositional phrases: Modification
- 4 Syntactic uses of the adpositional phrase
- 5 R-pronominalization and R-words
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Phonology
-
- General
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological Processes
- Assimilation
- Vowel nasalization
- Syllabic sonorants
- Final devoicing
- Fake geminates
- Vowel hiatus resolution
- Vowel reduction introduction
- Schwa deletion
- Schwa insertion
- /r/-deletion
- d-insertion
- {s/z}-insertion
- t-deletion
- Intrusive stop formation
- Breaking
- Vowel shortening
- h-deletion
- Replacement of the glide w
- Word stress
- Clitics
- Allomorphy
- Orthography of Frisian
- Morphology
- Inflection
- Word formation
- Derivation
- Prefixation
- Infixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixes
- Verbal suffixes
- Adjectival suffixes
- Adverbial suffixes
- Numeral suffixes
- Interjectional suffixes
- Onomastic suffixes
- Conversion
- Compositions
- Derivation
- Syntax
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Unergative and unaccusative subjects
- Evidentiality
- To-infinitival clauses
- Predication and noun incorporation
- Ellipsis
- Imperativus-pro-Infinitivo
- Expression of irrealis
- Embedded Verb Second
- Agreement
- Negation
- Nouns & Noun Phrases
- Classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Partitive noun constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Nominalised quantifiers
- Kind partitives
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Bare nominal attributions
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers and (pre)determiners
- Interrogative pronouns
- R-pronouns
- Syntactic uses
- Adjective Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification and degree quantification
- Comparison by degree
- Comparative
- Superlative
- Equative
- Attribution
- Agreement
- Attributive adjectives vs. prenominal elements
- Complex adjectives
- Noun ellipsis
- Co-occurring adjectives
- Predication
- Partitive adjective constructions
- Adverbial use
- Participles and infinitives
- Adposition Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Intransitive adpositions
- Predication
- Preposition stranding
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
-
- General
- Morphology
- Morphology
- 1 Word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 1.1.1 Compounds and their heads
- 1.1.2 Special types of compounds
- 1.1.2.1 Affixoids
- 1.1.2.2 Coordinative compounds
- 1.1.2.3 Synthetic compounds and complex pseudo-participles
- 1.1.2.4 Reduplicative compounds
- 1.1.2.5 Phrase-based compounds
- 1.1.2.6 Elative compounds
- 1.1.2.7 Exocentric compounds
- 1.1.2.8 Linking elements
- 1.1.2.9 Separable Complex Verbs and Particle Verbs
- 1.1.2.10 Noun Incorporation Verbs
- 1.1.2.11 Gapping
- 1.2 Derivation
- 1.3 Minor patterns of word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 2 Inflection
- 1 Word formation
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
- 0 Introduction to the AP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of APs
- 2 Complementation of APs
- 3 Modification and degree quantification of APs
- 4 Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative
- 5 Attribution of APs
- 6 Predication of APs
- 7 The partitive adjective construction
- 8 Adverbial use of APs
- 9 Participles and infinitives as APs
- Nouns and Noun Phrases (NPs)
- 0 Introduction to the NP
- 1 Characteristics and Classification of NPs
- 2 Complementation of NPs
- 3 Modification of NPs
- 3.1 Modification of NP by Determiners and APs
- 3.2 Modification of NP by PP
- 3.3 Modification of NP by adverbial clauses
- 3.4 Modification of NP by possessors
- 3.5 Modification of NP by relative clauses
- 3.6 Modification of NP in a cleft construction
- 3.7 Free relative clauses and selected interrogative clauses
- 4 Partitive noun constructions and constructions related to them
- 4.1 The referential partitive construction
- 4.2 The partitive construction of abstract quantity
- 4.3 The numerical partitive construction
- 4.4 The partitive interrogative construction
- 4.5 Adjectival, nominal and nominalised partitive quantifiers
- 4.6 Kind partitives
- 4.7 Partitive predication with a preposition
- 4.8 Bare nominal attribution
- 5 Articles and names
- 6 Pronouns
- 7 Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- 8 Interrogative pronouns
- 9 R-pronouns and the indefinite expletive
- 10 Syntactic functions of Noun Phrases
- Adpositions and Adpositional Phrases (PPs)
- 0 Introduction to the PP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of PPs
- 2 Complementation of PPs
- 3 Modification of PPs
- 4 Bare (intransitive) adpositions
- 5 Predication of PPs
- 6 Form and distribution of adpositions with respect to staticity and construction type
- 7 Adpositional complements and adverbials
- Verbs and Verb Phrases (VPs)
- 0 Introduction to the VP in Saterland Frisian
- 1 Characteristics and classification of verbs
- 2 Unergative and unaccusative subjects and the auxiliary of the perfect
- 3 Evidentiality in relation to perception and epistemicity
- 4 Types of to-infinitival constituents
- 5 Predication
- 5.1 The auxiliary of being and its selection restrictions
- 5.2 The auxiliary of going and its selection restrictions
- 5.3 The auxiliary of continuation and its selection restrictions
- 5.4 The auxiliary of coming and its selection restrictions
- 5.5 Modal auxiliaries and their selection restrictions
- 5.6 Auxiliaries of body posture and aspect and their selection restrictions
- 5.7 Transitive verbs of predication
- 5.8 The auxiliary of doing used as a semantically empty finite auxiliary
- 5.9 Supplementive predication
- 6 The verbal paradigm, irregularity and suppletion
- 7 Verb Second and the word order in main and embedded clauses
- 8 Various aspects of clause structure
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
-
- General
- Phonology
- Afrikaans phonology
- Segment inventory
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- The diphthongised long vowels /e/, /ø/ and /o/
- The unrounded mid-front vowel /ɛ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /ɑ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /a/
- The rounded mid-high back vowel /ɔ/
- The rounded high back vowel /u/
- The rounded and unrounded high front vowels /i/ and /y/
- The unrounded and rounded central vowels /ə/ and /œ/
- The diphthongs /əi/, /œy/ and /œu/
- Overview of Afrikaans consonants
- The bilabial plosives /p/ and /b/
- The alveolar plosives /t/ and /d/
- The velar plosives /k/ and /g/
- The bilabial nasal /m/
- The alveolar nasal /n/
- The velar nasal /ŋ/
- The trill /r/
- The lateral liquid /l/
- The alveolar fricative /s/
- The velar fricative /x/
- The labiodental fricatives /f/ and /v/
- The approximants /ɦ/, /j/ and /ʋ/
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- Word stress
- The phonetic properties of stress
- Primary stress on monomorphemic words in Afrikaans
- Background to primary stress in monomorphemes in Afrikaans
- Overview of the Main Stress Rule of Afrikaans
- The short vowels of Afrikaans
- Long vowels in monomorphemes
- Primary stress on diphthongs in monomorphemes
- Exceptions
- Stress shifts in place names
- Stress shift towards word-final position
- Stress pattern of reduplications
- Phonological processes
- Vowel related processes
- Consonant related processes
- Homorganic glide insertion
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Phonotactics
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Afrikaans syntax
- Nouns and noun phrases
- Characteristics of the NP
- Classification of nouns
- Complementation of NPs
- Modification of NPs
- Binominal and partitive constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Partitive constructions with nominalised quantifiers
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Binominal name constructions
- Binominal genitive constructions
- Bare nominal attribution
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- Syntactic uses of the noun phrase
- Adjectives and adjective phrases
- Characteristics and classification of the AP
- Complementation of APs
- Modification and Degree Quantification of APs
- Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative degree
- Attribution of APs
- Predication of APs
- The partitive adjective construction
- Adverbial use of APs
- Participles and infinitives as adjectives
- Verbs and verb phrases
- Characterisation and classification
- Argument structure
- Verb frame alternations
- Complements of non-main verbs
- Verb clusters
- Complement clauses
- Adverbial modification
- Word order in the clause: Introduction
- Word order in the clause: position of the finite Verb
- Word order in the clause: Clause-initial position
- Word order in the clause: Extraposition and right-dislocation in the postverbal field
- Word order in the middle field
- Emphatic constructions
- Adpositions and adposition phrases
Derounding of the marked rounded vowels is one of the most salient characteristics of Afrikaans. Age and gender are important factors in explaining differences with respect to the scale of derounding.
The derounding of Afrikaans marked rounded vowels has been in evidence since as early as the 18th century (Ponelis 1990). Le Roux and Pienaar (1927) mention this phenomenon in strongly negative terms: “plat … en sterk af te keur” (p.58) ( unrefined ... and to be avoided strongly). In contrast to this, De Villiers and Ponelis (1987) declare derounding in both koiné and Cape Vernacular Afrikaans as a given. Wissing (2010) states that derounding is a process that is an important distinguisher between Afrikaans on the one hand and the other Germanic languages, such as Dutch and Swedish, on the other; and adds that, even should derounding not be present, the difference between the rounded Afrikaans vowels and their Dutch and Swedish counterparts is notable: the acoustic distance between the rounded and unrounded vowels of these two Germanic languages is much greater than in Afrikaans.
Initially, the result of this process was thus viewed in strongly negative terms as being substandard; more recently, the presence thereof is described in neutral terms as a typical feature of Afrikaans and impacting on a number of vowels: thus the unrounding of /y/, /ø/ and /œ/ to resp. [i], [e] and [ə], as well as of the diphthong /œy/ to [əi], is among the most distinctive features of Afrikaans pronunciation compared to other Germanic languages. Derounding is most observable in casual speech, but is, by far, not restricted to this style. Even in formal language, as spoken by radio and television personalities, or on public occasions such as political speeches or sermons, it is readily heard. Lack of stress, as well as unaccentedness in connected speech, is conducive to, but not limited to, this phenomenon. Only in cases of extremely unknown and thus seldomly used words does the chance of derounding diminish.
Production and perception studies by Wissing(1994), (1995), Wissing and Van Dijkhorst(2006), Wissing(2011) and Wissing(2017) as well as further, unpublished investigations, all reveal the following picture of derounding among speakers of different genders and ages:
- Older speakers, especially those who are 70 years and older, do not deround as much as young speakers do.
- Younger speakers have considerably less difficulty in a discrimination task in which rounded ~ unrounded word pairs such as kleure colours and klere clothing are involved. In the relevant experiment such pairs were produced by both older speakers and younger speakers. In the latter case a RAP of only 0.43 was achieved; for productions by the older speakers nearly 1.0 was registered. These results point in the direction of the existence of two chronolects. This, in turn, could also be evidence for a diachronic shift in the case of the vowel system of Afrikaans. Compare also the section Age as a factor lower down for more specific information on this matter.
- Although derounding is present over a broad spectrum of modern Afrikaans, absolute neutralization of the contrast rounded ~ unrounded for these marked vowels has not yet taken place .
Some recent unpublished investigations into vowel derounding underscores the impression that derounding in Afrikaans is indeed a wide-spread phenomenon (For speech data sets that were used in these investigations, see Introduction to phonological processes).
In the following sections some of the most interesting findings from the analysis of these data sets regarding vowel derounding is presented.
Vowel derounding is a special type of phonological process in the sense that it is not particularly sensitive to phonetic context, that is, it commonly takes place even in monosyllabic words, thus not specifically in unstressed positions of multisyllabic words. An example of the former is the word nuus /nys/ news, pronounced in its unrounded form as [nis]. Of course, the known factors which enhance the application of phonological processes (see Introduction to phonological processes) are still relevant here. Therefore, for example, the rounded vowel /y/ in well-known and frequently used words like minuut /mənyt/ minute, uur /yr/ hour and musiek /mysik/ music is more likely to be unrounded than in less well-known and less-frequently used words such as immuun /imyn/ immune and vakuum /vɑkym/ vacuum. Speech style and rate are other contributing factors.
In the following sections a systematic description of derounding against this background is presented.
Wissing and Van Dijkhorst (2006) found strong evidence in favour of the view that the RAP of derounding is governed by speech rate, both in a reading task (a passage of prose) and in spontaneous speech (story telling). The three candidates for potential derounding were /y/, /ø/ and /œ/, as in, for example, the stimulus words uur hour, neus nose and bus bus. Derounding resulted in resp. [i], [iə] and [ə]. Two speaking modes were controlled for, rendering results for NormTemp and FastTemp (resp. referred to elsewhere as RAN and RAF). The results are indicative of a greater tendency for derounding to take place at a fast speaking rate (an RAP of 0.65 for RAF compared to 0.57 for RAN).
It is clear that derounding as a phonological process is sensitive to these factors. In the RSG data set, supposedly read in a formal albeit relaxed style, the function word deur /dør/ by, for example, occurs 267 times; of these, 76 were rounded, that is, a RAP of 0.7, which is very much in line with the RAP of the FastTemp mentioned in the previous section. Being a function word, deur is one of the most frequently used Afrikaans words. In contrast, the vowel of nuus, in the same data set, had an RAP of only 0.25 for derounding of (i.e. 4 out of 20 possible cases). This relative proportion of 267: 20 tokens of deur: nuus is in accordance with the number of occurrences of these two words in VivA's corpus of nearly 85 million words. In this corpus, deur appears 11777 times, compared to only 64 times for nuus. These findings strongly support the general assumption that frequency of usage is an important factor in predicting the application of phonological rules. Similarly, the probability of derounding, from /œy/ to [əi], in the case of the diphthong in the frequently used function word uit, is significantly higher than that in much less used and less known content words like snuit /snœyt/ snout or kruis /krœys/ cross.
A special case is that of place names ending in -burg /bœrx/ e.g. Johannesburg, Rustenburg, Sasolburg, Pietermaritzburg, Postmasburg, Vryburg, Vredenburg, all names occurring frequently every day during the Afrikaans national radio broadcaster, RSG's, weather reports. An inspection of the reading by one prominent RSG-presenter revealed that the realization of the u-vowel (i.e. the /œ/ phoneme) in these place names, but also in words like temperature temperature and minus id., is almost almost always unrounded i.e. [ə]. Words ending on -or behaves somewhat differently. On the one hand, derounding of the vowel in /ɔr/ regularly produces a [ə] as in doktor, motor, professor, junior and senior, but not in faktor, lektor, rektor and sektor – all loan words from English, and all of them with more or less equal frequency usage. doktor is especially interesting in that dokter – a physician – and doktor – a person holding a PhD-degree – are both titles, abbreviated as dr., so that it frequently leads to ambiguity. It could, therefore, be expected that doktor's vowel would not be prone to derounding.
A special case in support of the strong tendency to derounding is the pronunciation of the 2nd person singular pronoun u /y/ in Afrikaans. While it is hardly ever heard in casual Afrikaans, in formal instances it is still sometimes observable. In prayers, as is typically heard in sermons, u is frequently used. Interestingly, even in such extremely formal styles, derounding is not totally absent. This is particularly evident in the Afrikaans usage of young preachers. Wissing and Van Dijkhorst (2006) reports a RAP of 0.8 for derounding of u to [i] in the case of a 25-year old minister-in-training, in contrast to a RAP of <0.1 for a 65-year old.
Above-mentioned results regarding age as factor in the realm of derounding are supported by the findings of an unpublished study. In this study, 16 individuals divided into four age groups (20 – 39; 40 – 59; 60 – 79; 80+) and balanced as to gender, participated in a production test. Word pairs consisting of unrounded vowels and their rounded counterparts ( /i/ ~ /y/ (e.g. nies sneeze ~ nuus news); /ə/ ~ /œ/ (e.g. dis it's ~ dus thus), and /e/ ~ /ø/ (e.g. mees most ~ neus nose) were involved. Generally the oldest group of speakers succeeded best in distinguishing between especially /i/ and /y/, and between /ə/ and /œ/, followed by the groups 40 – 59, 60 – 79 and 20 – 39, in this order. Only /e/ ~ /ø/ rendered a somewhat mixed bag.
The results pertaining to the youngest group are underscored by another experiment, in which a group of twelve undergraduate students took part. Only three of them succeeded in making a clear distinction between /ə/ and /œ/; only two with respect to /i/ and /y/. Their performance regarding the third pair ( /e/ ~ /ø/) was somewhat better in that 50% of them were successful in this task.
No conclusive experimental results are available in this regard. It might be reasonable, however, to expect female speakers, generally considered to be leaders in language, and, for that matter, sound change, to exhibit the process of vowel derounding more than males do.
Rather scant information is available on the presence or absence of derounding in varieties other than Standard Afrikaans (see Introduction). Wissing (1994), (1995), (2011), (2017), in general, points out that the neutralization of the contrast rounded ~ unrounded, in the case of the marked non-back vowels, is variable, despite claims by Links (1989) that the marked rounded front vowels are rounded without exception in the relevant varieties. Wissing (2011) shows that this is clearly not true. Van Wyk (1983) states that these vowels are totally unrounded in pidginised Afrikaans, namely in so-called Black Afrikaans (Afrikaans spoken by black South Africans). See also De Wet (1993) and Meiring and Retief (1991) for a similar view.
- 1987Afrikaanse klankleer.Tafelberg
- 1993Swartafrikaans as niestandaardvariëteit van Afrikaans.South African Journal of Linguistics = Suid-Afrikaanse Tydskrif vir Taalkunde. Supplement 18170-188,
- 1927Afrikaanse fonetiek.Juta
- 1989So praat ons Namakwalanders.Tafelberg
- 1990Historiese klankleer van Afrikaans.ReeksUniversiteit van Stellenbosch
- 1991Funksionele Afrikaans.Academica
- 1983Gepidginiseerde Afrikaans.Bundels
- 1994Ontronding in wit en bruin Afrikaans.Literator15121-143,
- 1994Ontronding in wit en bruin Afrikaans.Literator15121-143,
- 1995Ontronding in twee variëteite van Afrikaans: 'n produksie- en persepsiestudie.Bundels
- 1995Ontronding in twee variëteite van Afrikaans: 'n produksie- en persepsiestudie.Bundels
- 2011Ontronding in Afrikaans / Derounding in Afrikaans.Tydskrif vir Geesteswetenskappe511-20,
- 2011Ontronding in Kharkamsafrikaans?
- 2011Ontronding in Kharkamsafrikaans?
- 2017Die vokaalstelsel van Genadendalse Afrikaans: ’n verkenning.
- 2006Is spreekstyl en spreektempo sinonieme?: 'n fonologiese ondersoek.Southern African Linguistics and Applied Language Studies24217-233
- 2006Is spreekstyl en spreektempo sinonieme?: 'n fonologiese ondersoek.Southern African Linguistics and Applied Language Studies24217-233
- 2006Is spreekstyl en spreektempo sinonieme?: 'n fonologiese ondersoek.Southern African Linguistics and Applied Language Studies24217-233
- 2010Oor die onstabiele klemtoongedrag van Afrikaanse reduplikasies.Southern African Linguistics and Applied Language Studies2813-23,
- 2017FonologieVan Schaik