- Dutch
- Frisian
- Saterfrisian
- Afrikaans
-
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological processes
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Word stress
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Monomorphemic words
- Diachronic aspects
- Generalizations on stress placement
- Default penultimate stress
- Lexical stress
- The closed penult restriction
- Final closed syllables
- The diphthong restriction
- Superheavy syllables (SHS)
- The three-syllable window
- Segmental restrictions
- Phonetic correlates
- Stress shifts in loanwords
- Quantity-sensitivity
- Secondary stress
- Vowel reduction in unstressed syllables
- Stress in complex words
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Accent & intonation
- Clitics
- Spelling
- Morphology
- Word formation
- Compounding
- Nominal compounds
- Verbal compounds
- Adjectival compounds
- Affixoids
- Coordinative compounds
- Synthetic compounds
- Reduplicative compounds
- Phrase-based compounds
- Elative compounds
- Exocentric compounds
- Linking elements
- Separable complex verbs (SCVs)
- Gapping of complex words
- Particle verbs
- Copulative compounds
- Derivation
- Numerals
- Derivation: inputs and input restrictions
- The meaning of affixes
- Non-native morphology
- Cohering and non-cohering affixes
- Prefixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixation: person nouns
- Conversion
- Pseudo-participles
- Bound forms
- Nouns
- Nominal prefixes
- Nominal suffixes
- -aal and -eel
- -aar
- -aard
- -aat
- -air
- -aris
- -ast
- Diminutives
- -dom
- -een
- -ees
- -el (nominal)
- -elaar
- -enis
- -er (nominal)
- -erd
- -erik
- -es
- -eur
- -euse
- ge...te
- -heid
- -iaan, -aan
- -ief
- -iek
- -ier
- -ier (French)
- -ière
- -iet
- -igheid
- -ij and allomorphs
- -ijn
- -in
- -ing
- -isme
- -ist
- -iteit
- -ling
- -oir
- -oot
- -rice
- -schap
- -schap (de)
- -schap (het)
- -sel
- -st
- -ster
- -t
- -tal
- -te
- -voud
- Verbs
- Adjectives
- Adverbs
- Univerbation
- Neo-classical word formation
- Construction-dependent morphology
- Morphological productivity
- Compounding
- Inflection
- Inflection and derivation
- Allomorphy
- The interface between phonology and morphology
- Word formation
- Syntax
- Preface and acknowledgements
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of verb phrases I:Argument structure
- 3 Projection of verb phrases II:Verb frame alternations
- Introduction
- 3.1. Main types
- 3.2. Alternations involving the external argument
- 3.3. Alternations of noun phrases and PPs
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.3.1.1. Dative alternation with aan-phrases (recipients)
- 3.3.1.2. Dative alternation with naar-phrases (goals)
- 3.3.1.3. Dative alternation with van-phrases (sources)
- 3.3.1.4. Dative alternation with bij-phrases (possessors)
- 3.3.1.5. Dative alternation with voor-phrases (benefactives)
- 3.3.1.6. Conclusion
- 3.3.1.7. Bibliographical notes
- 3.3.2. Accusative/PP alternations
- 3.3.3. Nominative/PP alternations
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.4. Some apparent cases of verb frame alternation
- 3.5. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of verb phrases IIIa:Selection of clauses/verb phrases
- 5 Projection of verb phrases IIIb:Argument and complementive clauses
- Introduction
- 5.1. Finite argument clauses
- 5.2. Infinitival argument clauses
- 5.3. Complementive clauses
- 6 Projection of verb phrases IIIc:Complements of non-main verbs
- 7 Projection of verb phrases IIId:Verb clusters
- 8 Projection of verb phrases IV: Adverbial modification
- 9 Word order in the clause I:General introduction
- 10 Word order in the clause II:Position of the finite verb (verb-first/second)
- 11 Word order in the clause III:Clause-initial position (wh-movement)
- Introduction
- 11.1. The formation of V1- and V2-clauses
- 11.2. Clause-initial position remains (phonetically) empty
- 11.3. Clause-initial position is filled
- 12 Word order in the clause IV:Postverbal field (extraposition)
- 13 Word order in the clause V: Middle field (scrambling)
- 14 Main-clause external elements
- Nouns and Noun Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of noun phrases I: complementation
- Introduction
- 2.1. General observations
- 2.2. Prepositional and nominal complements
- 2.3. Clausal complements
- 2.4. Bibliographical notes
- 3 Projection of noun phrases II: modification
- Introduction
- 3.1. Restrictive and non-restrictive modifiers
- 3.2. Premodification
- 3.3. Postmodification
- 3.3.1. Adpositional phrases
- 3.3.2. Relative clauses
- 3.3.3. Infinitival clauses
- 3.3.4. A special case: clauses referring to a proposition
- 3.3.5. Adjectival phrases
- 3.3.6. Adverbial postmodification
- 3.4. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of noun phrases III: binominal constructions
- Introduction
- 4.1. Binominal constructions without a preposition
- 4.2. Binominal constructions with a preposition
- 4.3. Bibliographical notes
- 5 Determiners: articles and pronouns
- Introduction
- 5.1. Articles
- 5.2. Pronouns
- 5.3. Bibliographical notes
- 6 Numerals and quantifiers
- 7 Pre-determiners
- Introduction
- 7.1. The universal quantifier al 'all' and its alternants
- 7.2. The pre-determiner heel 'all/whole'
- 7.3. A note on focus particles
- 7.4. Bibliographical notes
- 8 Syntactic uses of noun phrases
- Adjectives and Adjective Phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- 2 Projection of adjective phrases I: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adjective phrases II: Modification
- 4 Projection of adjective phrases III: Comparison
- 5 Attributive use of the adjective phrase
- 6 Predicative use of the adjective phrase
- 7 The partitive genitive construction
- 8 Adverbial use of the adjective phrase
- 9 Participles and infinitives: their adjectival use
- 10 Special constructions
- Adpositions and adpositional phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Introduction
- 1.1. Characterization of the category adposition
- 1.2. A formal classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3. A semantic classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3.1. Spatial adpositions
- 1.3.2. Temporal adpositions
- 1.3.3. Non-spatial/temporal prepositions
- 1.4. Borderline cases
- 1.5. Bibliographical notes
- 2 Projection of adpositional phrases: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adpositional phrases: Modification
- 4 Syntactic uses of the adpositional phrase
- 5 R-pronominalization and R-words
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Phonology
-
- General
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological Processes
- Assimilation
- Vowel nasalization
- Syllabic sonorants
- Final devoicing
- Fake geminates
- Vowel hiatus resolution
- Vowel reduction introduction
- Schwa deletion
- Schwa insertion
- /r/-deletion
- d-insertion
- {s/z}-insertion
- t-deletion
- Intrusive stop formation
- Breaking
- Vowel shortening
- h-deletion
- Replacement of the glide w
- Word stress
- Clitics
- Allomorphy
- Orthography of Frisian
- Morphology
- Inflection
- Word formation
- Derivation
- Prefixation
- Infixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixes
- Verbal suffixes
- Adjectival suffixes
- Adverbial suffixes
- Numeral suffixes
- Interjectional suffixes
- Onomastic suffixes
- Conversion
- Compositions
- Derivation
- Syntax
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Unergative and unaccusative subjects
- Evidentiality
- To-infinitival clauses
- Predication and noun incorporation
- Ellipsis
- Imperativus-pro-Infinitivo
- Expression of irrealis
- Embedded Verb Second
- Agreement
- Negation
- Nouns & Noun Phrases
- Classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Partitive noun constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Nominalised quantifiers
- Kind partitives
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Bare nominal attributions
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers and (pre)determiners
- Interrogative pronouns
- R-pronouns
- Syntactic uses
- Adjective Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification and degree quantification
- Comparison by degree
- Comparative
- Superlative
- Equative
- Attribution
- Agreement
- Attributive adjectives vs. prenominal elements
- Complex adjectives
- Noun ellipsis
- Co-occurring adjectives
- Predication
- Partitive adjective constructions
- Adverbial use
- Participles and infinitives
- Adposition Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Intransitive adpositions
- Predication
- Preposition stranding
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
-
- General
- Morphology
- Morphology
- 1 Word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 1.1.1 Compounds and their heads
- 1.1.2 Special types of compounds
- 1.1.2.1 Affixoids
- 1.1.2.2 Coordinative compounds
- 1.1.2.3 Synthetic compounds and complex pseudo-participles
- 1.1.2.4 Reduplicative compounds
- 1.1.2.5 Phrase-based compounds
- 1.1.2.6 Elative compounds
- 1.1.2.7 Exocentric compounds
- 1.1.2.8 Linking elements
- 1.1.2.9 Separable Complex Verbs and Particle Verbs
- 1.1.2.10 Noun Incorporation Verbs
- 1.1.2.11 Gapping
- 1.2 Derivation
- 1.3 Minor patterns of word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 2 Inflection
- 1 Word formation
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
- 0 Introduction to the AP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of APs
- 2 Complementation of APs
- 3 Modification and degree quantification of APs
- 4 Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative
- 5 Attribution of APs
- 6 Predication of APs
- 7 The partitive adjective construction
- 8 Adverbial use of APs
- 9 Participles and infinitives as APs
- Nouns and Noun Phrases (NPs)
- 0 Introduction to the NP
- 1 Characteristics and Classification of NPs
- 2 Complementation of NPs
- 3 Modification of NPs
- 3.1 Modification of NP by Determiners and APs
- 3.2 Modification of NP by PP
- 3.3 Modification of NP by adverbial clauses
- 3.4 Modification of NP by possessors
- 3.5 Modification of NP by relative clauses
- 3.6 Modification of NP in a cleft construction
- 3.7 Free relative clauses and selected interrogative clauses
- 4 Partitive noun constructions and constructions related to them
- 4.1 The referential partitive construction
- 4.2 The partitive construction of abstract quantity
- 4.3 The numerical partitive construction
- 4.4 The partitive interrogative construction
- 4.5 Adjectival, nominal and nominalised partitive quantifiers
- 4.6 Kind partitives
- 4.7 Partitive predication with a preposition
- 4.8 Bare nominal attribution
- 5 Articles and names
- 6 Pronouns
- 7 Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- 8 Interrogative pronouns
- 9 R-pronouns and the indefinite expletive
- 10 Syntactic functions of Noun Phrases
- Adpositions and Adpositional Phrases (PPs)
- 0 Introduction to the PP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of PPs
- 2 Complementation of PPs
- 3 Modification of PPs
- 4 Bare (intransitive) adpositions
- 5 Predication of PPs
- 6 Form and distribution of adpositions with respect to staticity and construction type
- 7 Adpositional complements and adverbials
- Verbs and Verb Phrases (VPs)
- 0 Introduction to the VP in Saterland Frisian
- 1 Characteristics and classification of verbs
- 2 Unergative and unaccusative subjects and the auxiliary of the perfect
- 3 Evidentiality in relation to perception and epistemicity
- 4 Types of to-infinitival constituents
- 5 Predication
- 5.1 The auxiliary of being and its selection restrictions
- 5.2 The auxiliary of going and its selection restrictions
- 5.3 The auxiliary of continuation and its selection restrictions
- 5.4 The auxiliary of coming and its selection restrictions
- 5.5 Modal auxiliaries and their selection restrictions
- 5.6 Auxiliaries of body posture and aspect and their selection restrictions
- 5.7 Transitive verbs of predication
- 5.8 The auxiliary of doing used as a semantically empty finite auxiliary
- 5.9 Supplementive predication
- 6 The verbal paradigm, irregularity and suppletion
- 7 Verb Second and the word order in main and embedded clauses
- 8 Various aspects of clause structure
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
-
- General
- Phonology
- Afrikaans phonology
- Segment inventory
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- The diphthongised long vowels /e/, /ø/ and /o/
- The unrounded mid-front vowel /ɛ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /ɑ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /a/
- The rounded mid-high back vowel /ɔ/
- The rounded high back vowel /u/
- The rounded and unrounded high front vowels /i/ and /y/
- The unrounded and rounded central vowels /ə/ and /œ/
- The diphthongs /əi/, /œy/ and /œu/
- Overview of Afrikaans consonants
- The bilabial plosives /p/ and /b/
- The alveolar plosives /t/ and /d/
- The velar plosives /k/ and /g/
- The bilabial nasal /m/
- The alveolar nasal /n/
- The velar nasal /ŋ/
- The trill /r/
- The lateral liquid /l/
- The alveolar fricative /s/
- The velar fricative /x/
- The labiodental fricatives /f/ and /v/
- The approximants /ɦ/, /j/ and /ʋ/
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- Word stress
- The phonetic properties of stress
- Primary stress on monomorphemic words in Afrikaans
- Background to primary stress in monomorphemes in Afrikaans
- Overview of the Main Stress Rule of Afrikaans
- The short vowels of Afrikaans
- Long vowels in monomorphemes
- Primary stress on diphthongs in monomorphemes
- Exceptions
- Stress shifts in place names
- Stress shift towards word-final position
- Stress pattern of reduplications
- Phonological processes
- Vowel related processes
- Consonant related processes
- Homorganic glide insertion
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Phonotactics
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Afrikaans syntax
- Nouns and noun phrases
- Characteristics of the NP
- Classification of nouns
- Complementation of NPs
- Modification of NPs
- Binominal and partitive constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Partitive constructions with nominalised quantifiers
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Binominal name constructions
- Binominal genitive constructions
- Bare nominal attribution
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- Syntactic uses of the noun phrase
- Adjectives and adjective phrases
- Characteristics and classification of the AP
- Complementation of APs
- Modification and Degree Quantification of APs
- Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative degree
- Attribution of APs
- Predication of APs
- The partitive adjective construction
- Adverbial use of APs
- Participles and infinitives as adjectives
- Verbs and verb phrases
- Characterisation and classification
- Argument structure
- Verb frame alternations
- Complements of non-main verbs
- Verb clusters
- Complement clauses
- Adverbial modification
- Word order in the clause: Introduction
- Word order in the clause: position of the finite Verb
- Word order in the clause: Clause-initial position
- Word order in the clause: Extraposition and right-dislocation in the postverbal field
- Word order in the middle field
- Emphatic constructions
- Adpositions and adposition phrases
The impersonal middle construction, which has not been studied much so far, is illustrated by means of the primed examples in (257). The construction owes its name to the fact that the subject is invariably the non-referential pronoun het'it'; replacing this pronoun by a referential one like the demonstrative dit'this' leads to ungrammaticality. Impersonal middles obligatorily contain an adverbial PP, as a result of which their meaning comes close to adjunct middles; it attributes properties to the instrument, the location or the time referred to by the nominal part of the adverbial phrase.
a. | Jan | snijdt | graag | met dat mes. | |
Jan | cuts | readily | with that knife |
a'. | Het/*Dit | snijdt | lekker | met dat mes. | |
it/this | cuts | nicely | with that knife |
b. | Peter | zit | vaak | op die stoel. | |
Peter | sits | often | on that chair |
b'. | Het/*Dit | zit lekker | op die stoel. | |
it/this | sits nicely | on that chair |
The claim that the adverbial PPs in (257) are obligatory does not mean that their omission results in ungrammaticality, but that the resulting structures in (258) are not impersonal middles; we are dealing with, respectively, an adjunct and a regular middle, as is clear from the fact that the pronoun het is referential and can thus be replaced by the demonstrative dit or a referential noun phrase.
a. | Het/Dit/Het krukje | zit | lekker. | |
it/this/the stool | sits | nicely |
b. | Het/Dit/Het vlees | snijdt | lekker. | |
it/this/the meat | cuts | easily |
The following subsections discuss the impersonal middle construction in more detail, subsection I shows that impersonal middle formation exhibits the same restriction on the input verb as adjunct middle formation. The fact that the two middle constructions are very close semantically is accounted for in Subsection II by showing that the non-referential subject pronoun het of the impersonal middle functions as an anticipatory pronoun that is coindexed with an adverbial phrase that can also be the counterpart of the subject in an adjunct middle, subsection III continues with a discussion of these adverbial adjuncts, subsection IV concludes with a discussion of the (implicit) experiencer introduced by the evaluative modifier.
Impersonal middles are like adjunct middles and unlike regular middles in that they can readily take an intransitive verb as input. This may again be attributed to the fact that the subject of the impersonal middle does not correspond to the direct object of the input verbs.
a. | Peter fietst | graag | op het fietspad. | |
Peter cycles | readily | on the bikeway | ||
'Peter likes to cycle on the bikeway.' |
a'. | Het | fietst | lekker | op het fietspad. | |
it | cycles | nicely | on the bikeway | ||
'It is nice to cycle on the bikeway.' |
b. | Peter fietst | op zijn nieuwe fiets. | |
Peter cycles | on his new bicycle | ||
'Peter is cycling on his new bicycle.' |
b'. | Het | fietst | lekker | op deze nieuwe fiets. | |
it | cycles | nicely | on this new bicycle | ||
'It is nice to cycle on this new bicycle.' |
The examples in (260) show that adjunct middles behave like regular middles and unlike passives in that the subject of the input verb cannot be expressed by means of an agentive door-phrase. Nevertheless, the notion of agent still seems to be implied; the evaluative modifier provides an assessment of some property of the subject in relation to the activity denoted by the verb, and thus indirectly introduces the notion of agentivity.
a. | * | Het | fietst | lekker | op het fietspad | door Peter. |
it | cycles | nicely | on the bikeway | by Peter |
b. | * | Het | fietst | lekker | op deze nieuwe fiets | door Peter. |
it | cycles | nicely | on this new bicycle | by Peter |
Transitive verbs can only be used as input for impersonal middle formation if they can be used as pseudo-intransitives; overtly realizing the objects in the middle constructions in the primed examples in (261) leads to unacceptability.
a. | Jan eet | (zijn lunch) | in een hoog tempo. | |
Jan eats | his lunch | at a high speed | ||
'Jan is eating his lunch at high speed.' |
a'. | Het | eet | (*lunch) | niet prettig | in een hoog tempo. | |
it | eats | lunch | not pleasantly | at a high speed | ||
'It isnʼt pleasant to eat at high speed.' |
b. | Jan | leest | graag | (romans) | op rustige middagen. | |
Jan | reads | readily | novels | on quiet afternoons | ||
'Jan likes to read (novels) on quiet afternoons.' |
b'. | Het | leest | (*romans) | het prettigst | op rustige middagen. | |
it | reads | novels | the most pleasant | on quiet afternoons | ||
'It is the most pleasant to read on quiet afternoons.' |
For the same reason, ditransitive verbs normally cannot be the input verb for impersonal middle formation; this is only (at best marginally) possible if the direct object can be omitted, as in the (b)-examples of (262).
a. | Marie geeft | het Rode Kruis | *(geld). | |
Marie gives | the Red Cross | money |
a'. | * | Het | geeft | het Rode Kruis | gemakkelijk | geld. |
it | gives | the Red Cross | easily | money |
b. | Marie geeft | (geld) | aan het Rode Kruis. | |
Marie gives | money | to the Red Cross |
b'. | Het | geeft | gemakkelijk | ?(*geld) | aan het Rode Kruis. | |
it | gives | easily | money | to the Red Cross |
Impersonal middle formation is normally not possible on the basis of unaccusative verbs, although we may need to make an exception for unaccusative verbs allowing a stage context reading.
a. | Jan | valt | prettiger | op een judomat | dan | op de vloer. | |
Jan | falls | more.pleasantly | on a judo.mat | than | on the floor |
a'. | ? | Het | valt | prettiger | op een judomat | dan | op de vloer. |
it | falls | more.pleasantly | on a judo.mat | than | on the floor | ||
'It is more pleasant to fall on a judo mat than on the floor.' |
b. | Oude officieren | sterven | in het bejaardenhuis. | |
old officers | die | in an old.peopleʼs.home |
b'. | ?? | Het | sterft | prettiger | in een bejaardenhuis | dan | op het slagveld. |
it | dies | more.pleasantly | in an old.peopleʼs.home | than | on the battlefield | ||
'It is more pleasant to die in an old peopleʼs home than on the battlefield.' |
The examples in (264) show that impersonal middles take the auxiliary hebben'to have' in the perfect tense; this also holds for middle verbs derived from unaccusative verbs like vallen, which normally take zijn.
a. | Het | heeft | altijd al | lekker | gefietst | op dit fietspad. | |
it | has | all along | nicely | cycled | on this bikeway | ||
'It has always been nice to cycle on this bikeway.' |
b. | Het | heeft | nog | nooit | prettig | gegeten | in een snel tempo. | |
it | has | prt | never | pleasantly | eaten | at a high speed | ||
'It has never been pleasant to eat at high speed.' |
c. | Het | ??heeft/*is | altijd al | beter | gevallen | op een judomat | dan op de vloer. | |
it | has/is | all along | better | fallen | on a judo.mat | than on the floor | ||
'It has always been more pleasant to fall on a judo mat than on the floor.' |
The discussion in the previous subsection has shown that impersonal and adjunct middles are similar in that they both take (pseudo-)intransitive verbs as input; cf. Section 3.2.2.3, sub I. Furthermore, we have seen that the two constructions are also very similar semantically. Let us therefore provisionally assume that impersonal middles are impersonal counterparts of adjunct middles; the non-referential pronoun het is an anticipatory pronoun that is associated with the adverbial PP which acts as the logical subject of the clause. The impersonal middles in (259) and (261) would then have the structures in (265), in which coindexing is used to express the proposed relation between the anticipatory pronoun het and the adverbial phrase; we refer the reader to Section A6.6 for a discussion of comparable copular constructions: Heti is warm [in de tropen]i'It is hot in the tropics'.
a. | Heti | fietst | lekker | [op het fietspad]i. | |
it | cycles | nicely | on the bikeway |
b. | Heti | fietst | lekker | [op deze nieuwe fiets]i. | |
it | cycles | nicely | on this new bicycle |
c. | Heti | eet | niet prettig | [in een hoog tempo]i. | |
it | eats | not pleasantly | at a high speed | ||
'It isnʼt pleasant to eat at high speed.' |
d. | Heti | leest | het prettigst | [op rustige middagen]i. | |
it | read | the most pleasant | on quiet afternoons |
The syntactic and semantic similarities between adjunct and impersonal middles can now be accounted for by assuming that subjects of adjunct middles correspond to adjuncts that can be coindexed with the anticipatory subject pronoun het in impersonal middle constructions. If this is on the right track, we correctly predict that impersonal middles are subject to restrictions similar to those on adjunct middles. Section 3.2.2.3, sub VC, has demonstrated, for example, that the unacceptability of examples such as (266b) shows that subjects of adjunct middles cannot correspond to the nominal part of a PP-complement. We therefore correctly predict that the same thing holds for the impersonal middle in (266c). Observe that the latter example improves considerably if we add a locational adverbial phrase, as in (266c'), but this is due to the fact that the anticipatory pronoun can then be construed with the adverbial phrase.
a. | Jan kijkt | graag | naar schilderijen. | |
Jan looks | readily | to paintings | ||
'Jan likes to look at paintings.' |
b. | * | Schilderijen | kijken | prettig. |
paintings | look | pleasantly |
c. | * | Heti | kijkt | prettig | [naar schilderijen]i. |
it | looks | pleasantly | at paintings |
c'. | ? | Heti | kijkt | [in dat museum]i | prettig | naar schilderijen. |
it | looks | in that museum | pleasantly | at paintings |
Section 3.2.2.3, sub VC, also argued that the subject of an adjunct middle cannot correspond to the nominal part of a predicative PP. This was illustrated by means of the examples in (255), repeated here as (267), which show that the adverbial and predicative PPs can be distinguished by their meaning: an adverbial PP simply indicates where the activity denoted by the verb takes place, whereas the predicative PP refers to the new location that the subject of the clause obtains by performing the action denoted by the verb.
a. | Jan heeft | op de trampoline | gesprongen. | adverbial PP | |
Jan has | on the trampoline | jumped | |||
'Jan has jumped on the trampoline.' |
b. | Jan is op de trampoline | gesprongen. | predicative PP | |
Jan is onto the trampoline | jumped | |||
'Jan has jumped onto the trampoline.' |
The subject in the adjunct middle construction in (256), repeated here as (268a), clearly corresponds to the adverbial PP: it is the jumping on the trampoline that is claimed to be nice, not the jumping onto the trampoline. Example (268b) shows that the same thing holds for the corresponding impersonal middle.
a. | De trampoline springt | lekker. | |
the trampoline jumps | nicely | ||
Available reading: 'It is nice to jump on the trampoline.' | |||
Impossible reading: 'It is nice to jump onto the trampoline.' |
b. | Heti | springt | lekker | [op de trampoline]i. | |
it | jumps | nicely | on the trampoline | ||
Available reading: 'It is nice to jump on the trampoline.' | |||||
Impossible reading: 'It is nice to jump onto the trampoline.' |
We conclude from the discussion above that anticipatory pronouns in impersonal middles can only be coindexed with adverbial phrases. This may have interesting results for cases in which the status of a certain PP is unclear, like the locational PP that co-occurs with the verb wonen'to live' in example (269a). Given that this PP is obligatory, it has been suggested that it is a PP-complement or a complementive selected by the verb. The fact that this example has an impersonal middle counterpart strongly suggests, however, that the PP is a regular adverbial phrase.
a. | Jan | woont | *(in Amsterdam). | |
Jan | lives | in Amsterdam |
b. | Heti | woont | prettig | [in Amsterdam]i. | |
it | lives | pleasantly | in Amsterdam |
In relation to the hypothesis that the non-referential pronoun het in the impersonal middle functions as an anticipatory pronoun associated with an adverbial PP that acts as the logical subject of the clause, it may be useful to note that impersonal middles are special in that their nominalizations can be readily used as nominal predicates. The resulting copular constructions in (270) are likewise impersonal in nature; the subject pronoun het cannot be replaced by, e.g., a deictic pronoun and seems to function as an anticipatory pronoun co-indexed with the adverbial PP; we refer the reader again to Section A6.6 for a discussion of comparable constructions.
a. | dat | heti | lekker | fietsen | is | [op het fietspad]i. | |
that | it | nicely | cycling | is | on the bikeway | ||
'that cycling is nice on the bikeway.' |
b. | dat | heti | lekker | fietsen is | [op deze nieuwe fiets]i. | |
that | it | nicely | cycling is | on this new bicycle | ||
'that cycling is nice on this new bicycle.' |
To conclude this discussion on the anticipatory subject pronoun het of the impersonal middle construction, we want to note that impersonal middles do not have corresponding constructions in which participles are used as attributive modifiers or secondary predicates. This is, of course, not due to the verb but to the fact that the subject pronoun het is non-referential and can therefore not be modified or function as an argument of a predicate.
The previous subsection suggested that adjunct middles always have an impersonal middle counterpart. It does not seem to be the case, however, that the inverse holds as well; impersonal middles seem possible with a somewhat wider range of adjunct types than adjunct middles. This will become clear by comparing the examples in (271); the non-middle construction in (271a) has an impersonal but not an adjunct middle counterpart.
a. | Peter | eet | prettig | aan die tafel. | |
Peter | eats | pleasantly | at that table |
b. | Heti | eet | prettig | [aan die tafel]i. | |
it | eats | pleasantly | at that table |
b'. | *? | Die tafel eet | prettig. |
that table eats | pleasantly |
The examples in (272) suggest that the contrast in acceptability between impersonal and adjunct middles can at least sometimes be related to meaning. The nominal part of the adverbial met-PP in (272a) can readily be used as the subject of an adjunct middle, whereas the nominal part of the zonder-PP in (272b) cannot. This is clearly related to the fact that the implicit negation expressed by zonder is irrecoverably lost in the adjunct middle in (272b''). Note that Zonder helm rijdt lekker is more or less acceptable; this is, however, not a middle construction, but a construction with a PP subject.
a. | Jan rijdt | altijd | met autohandschoenen. | |
Jan drives | always | with car.gloves |
a'. | Het | rijdt | prettig | met autohandschoenen. | |
it | drives | pleasantly | with car.gloves |
a''. | Autohandschoenen | rijden | prettig. | |
car.gloves | drive | pleasantly |
b. | Jan | rijdt | altijd | zonder | helm. | |
Jan | drives | always | without | a.helmet |
b'. | Het | rijdt | prettiger | zonder | helm. | |
it | drives | more.pleasantly | without | a.helmet |
b''. | # | Een helm | rijdt | prettiger. |
a helmet | drive | more.pleasantly |
In other cases, it is less clear what determines the contrast, although the examples in (273) suggest that the nominal part of the adjunct must refer to an entity/entities that is/are in some conventional relation to the activity denoted by the verb in order to be able to appear as the subject of an adjunct middle.
a. | Het | schrijft | lekker | op dit papier/bij deze lamp. | |
it | writes | nicely | on this paper/near this lamp |
a'. | Dit papier/??Deze lamp | schrijft | lekker. | |
this paper/this lamp | writes | nicely |
b. | Het breit | lekker | met deze naalden/deze regen. | |
it knits | pleasantly | with these needles/this rain |
b'. | Deze naalden/??Deze regen | breit | lekker. | |
these needles/this rain | knits | pleasantly |
Ackema & Schoorlemmer (1994/2006:169-171) suggest that the contrast may also be related to the adjunct's ability to undergo preposition stranding; see the examples in (274).
a. | het papier | waar | Jan op | schrijft | |
the paper | that | Jan on | writes | ||
'the paper Jan is writing on' |
a'. | ? | de lamp | waar | Jan bij | schrijft |
the lamp | that | Jan near | writes | ||
'the lamp near which Jan is writing' |
b. | de naalden | waar | ik | mee | brei | |
the needles | that | I | with | knit | ||
'the needles Iʼm knitting with' |
b'. | * | de regen | waar | ik | mee | brei |
the rain | that | I | during | knits | ||
'the rain during which Iʼm knitting' |
They further suggest that the subject of adjunct middles corresponds to argument-PPs, not adjuncts, which would make the adjunct middle into a kind of regular middle. However, this runs afoul of the observation from Section 3.2.2.2, sub IA, that subjects of regular middles never correspond to the nominal part of a run-of-the mill PP-complement of the input verb.
Impersonal middles normally require the presence of an evaluative modifier like gemakkelijk'easily' or others listed in (170), although, just as in the case of adjunct middles, impersonal middles sometimes also occur with adjectives like snel'fast' and vlot'smoothly' that do not select an experiencer voor-phrase; compare the impersonal construction in (275b) with example (248b), repeated here as (275a).
a. | Deze weg | rijdt | snel/vlot. | |
this road | drives | fast/smoothly |
b. | Het | rijdt | snel/vlot | op deze weg. | |
it | drives | fast/smoothly | on this road |
The examples in (276) show that the experiencer selected by adjectives like gemakkelijk'easily' normally cannot be overtly realized (with the same proviso made in Section 3.2.2.2, sub IC, for the regular middle).
a. | ?? | Het | zit | voor iedereen | lekker | op die stoel. |
it | sits | for everybody | nicely | in that chair |
b. | ?? | Het | snijdt | voor iedereen | prettig | met dat mes. |
it | cuts | for everybody | pleasantly | with that knife |
c. | ?? | Het | werkt | voor iedereen | prettig | in dat tempo. |
it | works | for everybody | pleasantly | in that tempo |
The examples in (277) show that, as in the case of adjunct middles, the evaluative modifier can at least marginally be left out if the negative adverb niet'not' is present, if the verb is emphatically accented, or if the evaluation is expressed by some other means.
a. | ? | Het | schrijft | niet | op dit papier. |
it | writes | not | on this paper |
b. | ? | Het | breit | met deze wol. | Pfff! |
it | needles | with this wool | Pfff |
c. | ? | Het | werkt | als | een trein | in de vakantie. |
it | works | like | a train | in the vacation |
- 1994The middle construction and the syntax-semantics interfaceLingua9359-90
- 2006MiddlesEveraert, Martin & Riemsdijk, Henk van (eds.)The Blackwell companion to syntax3Malden, MA/OxfordBlackwell Publishing131-203