- Dutch1
- Frisian
- Saterfrisian
- Afrikaans
-
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological processes
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Word stress
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Monomorphemic words
- Diachronic aspects
- Generalizations on stress placement
- Default penultimate stress
- Lexical stress
- The closed penult restriction
- Final closed syllables
- The diphthong restriction
- Superheavy syllables (SHS)
- The three-syllable window
- Segmental restrictions
- Phonetic correlates
- Stress shifts in loanwords
- Quantity-sensitivity
- Secondary stress
- Vowel reduction in unstressed syllables
- Stress in complex words
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Accent & intonation
- Clitics
- Spelling
- Morphology
- Word formation
- Compounding
- Nominal compounds
- Verbal compounds
- Adjectival compounds
- Affixoids
- Coordinative compounds
- Synthetic compounds
- Reduplicative compounds
- Phrase-based compounds
- Elative compounds
- Exocentric compounds
- Linking elements
- Separable complex verbs (SCVs)
- Gapping of complex words
- Particle verbs
- Copulative compounds
- Derivation
- Numerals
- Derivation: inputs and input restrictions
- The meaning of affixes
- Non-native morphology
- Cohering and non-cohering affixes
- Prefixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixation: person nouns
- Conversion
- Pseudo-participles
- Bound forms
- Nouns
- Nominal prefixes
- Nominal suffixes
- -aal and -eel
- -aar
- -aard
- -aat
- -air
- -aris
- -ast
- Diminutives
- -dom
- -een
- -ees
- -el (nominal)
- -elaar
- -enis
- -er (nominal)
- -erd
- -erik
- -es
- -eur
- -euse
- ge...te
- -heid
- -iaan, -aan
- -ief
- -iek
- -ier
- -ier (French)
- -ière
- -iet
- -igheid
- -ij and allomorphs
- -ijn
- -in
- -ing
- -isme
- -ist
- -iteit
- -ling
- -oir
- -oot
- -rice
- -schap
- -schap (de)
- -schap (het)
- -sel
- -st
- -ster
- -t
- -tal
- -te
- -voud
- Verbs
- Adjectives
- Adverbs
- Univerbation
- Neo-classical word formation
- Construction-dependent morphology
- Morphological productivity
- Compounding
- Inflection
- Inflection and derivation
- Allomorphy
- The interface between phonology and morphology
- Word formation
- Syntax
- Preface and acknowledgements
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of verb phrases I:Argument structure
- 3 Projection of verb phrases II:Verb frame alternations
- Introduction
- 3.1. Main types
- 3.2. Alternations involving the external argument
- 3.3. Alternations of noun phrases and PPs
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.3.1.1. Dative alternation with aan-phrases (recipients)
- 3.3.1.2. Dative alternation with naar-phrases (goals)
- 3.3.1.3. Dative alternation with van-phrases (sources)
- 3.3.1.4. Dative alternation with bij-phrases (possessors)
- 3.3.1.5. Dative alternation with voor-phrases (benefactives)
- 3.3.1.6. Conclusion
- 3.3.1.7. Bibliographical notes
- 3.3.2. Accusative/PP alternations
- 3.3.3. Nominative/PP alternations
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.4. Some apparent cases of verb frame alternation
- 3.5. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of verb phrases IIIa:Selection of clauses/verb phrases
- 5 Projection of verb phrases IIIb:Argument and complementive clauses
- Introduction
- 5.1. Finite argument clauses
- 5.2. Infinitival argument clauses
- 5.3. Complementive clauses
- 6 Projection of verb phrases IIIc:Complements of non-main verbs
- 7 Projection of verb phrases IIId:Verb clusters
- 8 Projection of verb phrases IV: Adverbial modification
- 9 Word order in the clause I:General introduction
- 10 Word order in the clause II:Position of the finite verb (verb-first/second)
- 11 Word order in the clause III:Clause-initial position (wh-movement)
- Introduction
- 11.1. The formation of V1- and V2-clauses
- 11.2. Clause-initial position remains (phonetically) empty
- 11.3. Clause-initial position is filled
- 12 Word order in the clause IV:Postverbal field (extraposition)
- 13 Word order in the clause V: Middle field (scrambling)
- 14 Main-clause external elements
- Nouns and Noun Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of noun phrases I: complementation
- Introduction
- 2.1. General observations
- 2.2. Prepositional and nominal complements
- 2.3. Clausal complements
- 2.4. Bibliographical notes
- 3 Projection of noun phrases II: modification
- Introduction
- 3.1. Restrictive and non-restrictive modifiers
- 3.2. Premodification
- 3.3. Postmodification
- 3.3.1. Adpositional phrases
- 3.3.2. Relative clauses
- 3.3.3. Infinitival clauses
- 3.3.4. A special case: clauses referring to a proposition
- 3.3.5. Adjectival phrases
- 3.3.6. Adverbial postmodification
- 3.4. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of noun phrases III: binominal constructions
- Introduction
- 4.1. Binominal constructions without a preposition
- 4.2. Binominal constructions with a preposition
- 4.3. Bibliographical notes
- 5 Determiners: articles and pronouns
- Introduction
- 5.1. Articles
- 5.2. Pronouns
- 5.3. Bibliographical notes
- 6 Numerals and quantifiers
- 7 Pre-determiners
- Introduction
- 7.1. The universal quantifier al 'all' and its alternants
- 7.2. The pre-determiner heel 'all/whole'
- 7.3. A note on focus particles
- 7.4. Bibliographical notes
- 8 Syntactic uses of noun phrases
- Adjectives and Adjective Phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- 2 Projection of adjective phrases I: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adjective phrases II: Modification
- 4 Projection of adjective phrases III: Comparison
- 5 Attributive use of the adjective phrase
- 6 Predicative use of the adjective phrase
- 7 The partitive genitive construction
- 8 Adverbial use of the adjective phrase
- 9 Participles and infinitives: their adjectival use
- 10 Special constructions
- Adpositions and adpositional phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Introduction
- 1.1. Characterization of the category adposition
- 1.2. A formal classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3. A semantic classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3.1. Spatial adpositions
- 1.3.2. Temporal adpositions
- 1.3.3. Non-spatial/temporal prepositions
- 1.4. Borderline cases
- 1.5. Bibliographical notes
- 2 Projection of adpositional phrases: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adpositional phrases: Modification
- 4 Syntactic uses of the adpositional phrase
- 5 R-pronominalization and R-words
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Phonology
-
- General
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological Processes
- Assimilation
- Vowel nasalization
- Syllabic sonorants
- Final devoicing
- Fake geminates
- Vowel hiatus resolution
- Vowel reduction introduction
- Schwa deletion
- Schwa insertion
- /r/-deletion
- d-insertion
- {s/z}-insertion
- t-deletion
- Intrusive stop formation
- Breaking
- Vowel shortening
- h-deletion
- Replacement of the glide w
- Word stress
- Clitics
- Allomorphy
- Orthography of Frisian
- Morphology
- Inflection
- Word formation
- Derivation
- Prefixation
- Infixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixes
- Verbal suffixes
- Adjectival suffixes
- Adverbial suffixes
- Numeral suffixes
- Interjectional suffixes
- Onomastic suffixes
- Conversion
- Compositions
- Derivation
- Syntax
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Unergative and unaccusative subjects
- Evidentiality
- To-infinitival clauses
- Predication and noun incorporation
- Ellipsis
- Imperativus-pro-Infinitivo
- Expression of irrealis
- Embedded Verb Second
- Agreement
- Negation
- Nouns & Noun Phrases
- Classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Partitive noun constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Nominalised quantifiers
- Kind partitives
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Bare nominal attributions
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers and (pre)determiners
- Interrogative pronouns
- R-pronouns
- Syntactic uses
- Adjective Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification and degree quantification
- Comparison by degree
- Comparative
- Superlative
- Equative
- Attribution
- Agreement
- Attributive adjectives vs. prenominal elements
- Complex adjectives
- Noun ellipsis
- Co-occurring adjectives
- Predication
- Partitive adjective constructions
- Adverbial use
- Participles and infinitives
- Adposition Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Intransitive adpositions
- Predication
- Preposition stranding
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
-
- General
- Morphology
- Morphology
- 1 Word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 1.1.1 Compounds and their heads
- 1.1.2 Special types of compounds
- 1.1.2.1 Affixoids
- 1.1.2.2 Coordinative compounds
- 1.1.2.3 Synthetic compounds and complex pseudo-participles
- 1.1.2.4 Reduplicative compounds
- 1.1.2.5 Phrase-based compounds
- 1.1.2.6 Elative compounds
- 1.1.2.7 Exocentric compounds
- 1.1.2.8 Linking elements
- 1.1.2.9 Separable Complex Verbs and Particle Verbs
- 1.1.2.10 Noun Incorporation Verbs
- 1.1.2.11 Gapping
- 1.2 Derivation
- 1.3 Minor patterns of word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 2 Inflection
- 1 Word formation
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
- 0 Introduction to the AP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of APs
- 2 Complementation of APs
- 3 Modification and degree quantification of APs
- 4 Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative
- 5 Attribution of APs
- 6 Predication of APs
- 7 The partitive adjective construction
- 8 Adverbial use of APs
- 9 Participles and infinitives as APs
- Nouns and Noun Phrases (NPs)
- 0 Introduction to the NP
- 1 Characteristics and Classification of NPs
- 2 Complementation of NPs
- 3 Modification of NPs
- 3.1 Modification of NP by Determiners and APs
- 3.2 Modification of NP by PP
- 3.3 Modification of NP by adverbial clauses
- 3.4 Modification of NP by possessors
- 3.5 Modification of NP by relative clauses
- 3.6 Modification of NP in a cleft construction
- 3.7 Free relative clauses and selected interrogative clauses
- 4 Partitive noun constructions and constructions related to them
- 4.1 The referential partitive construction
- 4.2 The partitive construction of abstract quantity
- 4.3 The numerical partitive construction
- 4.4 The partitive interrogative construction
- 4.5 Adjectival, nominal and nominalised partitive quantifiers
- 4.6 Kind partitives
- 4.7 Partitive predication with a preposition
- 4.8 Bare nominal attribution
- 5 Articles and names
- 6 Pronouns
- 7 Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- 8 Interrogative pronouns
- 9 R-pronouns and the indefinite expletive
- 10 Syntactic functions of Noun Phrases
- Adpositions and Adpositional Phrases (PPs)
- 0 Introduction to the PP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of PPs
- 2 Complementation of PPs
- 3 Modification of PPs
- 4 Bare (intransitive) adpositions
- 5 Predication of PPs
- 6 Form and distribution of adpositions with respect to staticity and construction type
- 7 Adpositional complements and adverbials
- Verbs and Verb Phrases (VPs)
- 0 Introduction to the VP in Saterland Frisian
- 1 Characteristics and classification of verbs
- 2 Unergative and unaccusative subjects and the auxiliary of the perfect
- 3 Evidentiality in relation to perception and epistemicity
- 4 Types of to-infinitival constituents
- 5 Predication
- 5.1 The auxiliary of being and its selection restrictions
- 5.2 The auxiliary of going and its selection restrictions
- 5.3 The auxiliary of continuation and its selection restrictions
- 5.4 The auxiliary of coming and its selection restrictions
- 5.5 Modal auxiliaries and their selection restrictions
- 5.6 Auxiliaries of body posture and aspect and their selection restrictions
- 5.7 Transitive verbs of predication
- 5.8 The auxiliary of doing used as a semantically empty finite auxiliary
- 5.9 Supplementive predication
- 6 The verbal paradigm, irregularity and suppletion
- 7 Verb Second and the word order in main and embedded clauses
- 8 Various aspects of clause structure
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
-
- General
- Phonology
- Afrikaans phonology
- Segment inventory
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- The diphthongised long vowels /e/, /ø/ and /o/
- The unrounded mid-front vowel /ɛ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /ɑ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /a/
- The rounded mid-high back vowel /ɔ/
- The rounded high back vowel /u/
- The rounded and unrounded high front vowels /i/ and /y/
- The unrounded and rounded central vowels /ə/ and /œ/
- The diphthongs /əi/, /œy/ and /œu/
- Overview of Afrikaans consonants
- The bilabial plosives /p/ and /b/
- The alveolar plosives /t/ and /d/
- The velar plosives /k/ and /g/
- The bilabial nasal /m/
- The alveolar nasal /n/
- The velar nasal /ŋ/
- The trill /r/
- The lateral liquid /l/
- The alveolar fricative /s/
- The velar fricative /x/
- The labiodental fricatives /f/ and /v/
- The approximants /ɦ/, /j/ and /ʋ/
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- Word stress
- The phonetic properties of stress
- Primary stress on monomorphemic words in Afrikaans
- Background to primary stress in monomorphemes in Afrikaans
- Overview of the Main Stress Rule of Afrikaans
- The short vowels of Afrikaans
- Long vowels in monomorphemes
- Primary stress on diphthongs in monomorphemes
- Exceptions
- Stress shifts in place names
- Stress shift towards word-final position
- Stress pattern of reduplications
- Phonological processes
- Vowel related processes
- Consonant related processes
- Homorganic glide insertion
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Phonotactics
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Afrikaans syntax
- Nouns and noun phrases
- Characteristics of the NP
- Classification of nouns
- Complementation of NPs
- Modification of NPs
- Binominal and partitive constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Partitive constructions with nominalised quantifiers
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Binominal name constructions
- Binominal genitive constructions
- Bare nominal attribution
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- Syntactic uses of the noun phrase
- Adjectives and adjective phrases
- Characteristics and classification of the AP
- Complementation of APs
- Modification and Degree Quantification of APs
- Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative degree
- Attribution of APs
- Predication of APs
- The partitive adjective construction
- Adverbial use of APs
- Participles and infinitives as adjectives
- Verbs and verb phrases
- Characterisation and classification
- Argument structure
- Verb frame alternations
- Complements of non-main verbs
- Verb clusters
- Complement clauses
- Adverbial modification
- Word order in the clause: Introduction
- Word order in the clause: position of the finite Verb
- Word order in the clause: Clause-initial position
- Word order in the clause: Extraposition and right-dislocation in the postverbal field
- Word order in the middle field
- Emphatic constructions
- Adpositions and adposition phrases
Non-main verbs differ from main verbs: they do not denote states of affairs, but express additional (temporal, modal, aspectual, etc.) information about states of affairs denoted by main verbs. This implies that non-main verbs are normally accompanied by the projection of a main verb. Moreover, constructions with non-main verbs are characterized by the fact that the main verbs in them are never finite. The examples in (65) also show that the form of the non-finite main verb depends on the type of non-main verb: perfect and passive auxiliaries, for example, combine with past/passive participles, modal/aspectual verbs combine with bare infinitivals, and semi-aspectual verbs combine with te-infinitivals.
a. | Jan heeft | dat boek | gelezen. | perfect auxiliary | |
Jan has | that book | read | |||
'Jan has read that book.' |
b. | Het boek | werd | me | (door Peter) | toegestuurd. | passive auxiliary | |
the book | was | me | by Peter | prt.-sent | |||
'The book was sent to me (by Peter).' |
c. | Jan wil/gaat | dat boek | kopen. | modal/aspectual verb | |
Jan wants/goes | that book | buy | |||
'Jan wants/is going to buy that book.' |
d. | Jan zit | dat boek | te lezen. | semi-aspectual verb | |
Jan sits | that book | to read | |||
'Jan is reading that book.' |
Although the set of non-main verbs traditionally assumed is substantially larger than the four groups mentioned in (65), we will confine ourselves to these verbs for the purpose of illustration; Section 5.2 will provide a more exhaustive discussion.
Auxiliaries like hebben and zijn are temporal in the sense that the perfect-tense constructions they are part of situate the state of affairs prior to a specific point in time. Example (66a), for instance, situates the arrival of Marie prior to the speech time (which is the default value), as the fact that it can be modified by the time adverbial gisteren'yesterday' but not by the time adverbial morgen'tomorrow' makes quite clear. In addition, perfect-tense constructions may under certain conditions also have aspectual implications by expressing that the state of affairs denoted by the main verb is completed in the sense that some logically implied endpoint has been reached: example (66b), for instance, can only be used when Jan has told the full story. We refer the reader to Section 1.5.1 for a more detailed discussion of the semantics of the perfect tense.
a. | Marie is (gisteren/*morgen) | gearriveerd. | |
Marie is yesterday/tomorrow | arrived | ||
'Marie arrived/Marie arrived yesterday.' |
b. | Jan heeft | me | het verhaal | (gisteren/*morgen) | verteld. | |
Jan has | me | the story | yesterday/tomorrow | told | ||
'Jan has told me the story (yesterday).' |
Participles are also used in combination with the auxiliaries worden'to be' and zijn'to have been' in regular passive constructions like (67a&b) and the auxiliary krijgen'to get' in so-called krijgen-passive constructions such as (67c).
a. | Het boek | werd | me | (door Peter) | toegestuurd. | |
the book | was | me | by Peter | prt.-sent | ||
'The book was sent to me (by Peter).' |
b. | Het boek | is | me | (door Peter) | toegestuurd. | |
the book | has.been | me | by Peter | prt.-sent | ||
'The book has been sent to me (by Peter).' |
c. | Ik | kreeg | het boek | (door Peter) | toegestuurd. | |
I | got | the book | by Peter | prt.-sent | ||
'I was sent the book (by Peter).' |
Note in passing that the auxiliary verb zijn in (67b) is sometimes analyzed not as a passive but as a perfect auxiliary given that the passive participle geworden can at least marginally be added to such examples. If correct, this means that worden and krijgen would exhaust the set of passive auxiliaries, but see Section 6.2.2 for a potential problem for this conclusion.
That the auxiliaries discussed in this section are only instrumental in creating perfect tense or passive constructions immediately accounts for the fact that they cannot be used as heads of clauses (although zijn'to be' and worden'to become' do occur as copulas, and hebben'to have' and krijgen'to get' can also be used as main verbs of possession).
The examples in (68) show that modal and aspectual verbs like willen and gaan differ from temporal and passive auxiliaries in that they do not combine with participles but require the main verb to take the form of a bare infinitive.
a. | Jan wil | dat boek | morgen | kopen. | |
Jan wants | that book | tomorrow | buy | ||
'Jan wants to buy that book tomorrow.' |
b. | Jan gaat | morgen | dat boek | kopen. | |
Jan goes | tomorrow | that book | buy | ||
'Jan is going to buy that book tomorrow.' |
The primeless examples in (69) show that modal and aspectual verbs also differ from main verbs in that they exhibit the infinitivus-pro-participio (IPP) effect; they do not take the form of a participle in perfect-tense constructions, but of an infinitive. The primed examples have been added to show that willen and gaan do appear as participles are used as main verbs.
a. | Jan heeft | dat boek | altijd | al | willen/*gewild | kopen. | |
Jan has | that book | always | already | want/wanted | buy | ||
'Jan has always wanted to buy that book.' |
a'. | Jan heeft | dat boek | altijd | al | gewild/*willen. | |
Jan has | that book | always | already | wanted/want | ||
'Jan has always wanted to have that book.' |
b. | Jan is | dat boek | gaan/*gegaan | kopen. | |
Jan has | that book | go/gone | buy | ||
'Jan has gone to buy that book.' |
b'. | Jan is naar de winkel | gegaan/*gaan. | |
Jan is to the shop | gone/go | ||
'Jan has gone to the shop.' |
If modal and aspectual verbs supplement the event expressed by the main verb with specific modal/aspectual information, we expect that these verbs cannot be used without a main verb. This is indeed borne out in the case of the aspectual verbs, but not in the case of the modal verbs: the (a)-examples in (70) show that the string een ijsje kopen can be pronominalized by means of het'it' or dat'that'; the (b)-examples show that this is not possible with aspectual verbs (although speakers do accept left dislocation constructions like Een ijsje kopen, dat gaan we zeker!'Buying ice cream we certainly will!'; we refer the reader to Section 4.6, sub II, for reasons for assuming that this does not involve pronominalization).
a. | Jan wil | [een ijsje | kopen]. | |
Jan wants | an ice.cream | buy | ||
'Jan wants to buy an ice cream.' |
a'. | Jan wil | het/dat. | |
Jan wants | it/that |
b. | Jan gaat | [een ijsje | kopen]. | |
Jan goes | an ice.cream | buy | ||
'Jan is going to buy an ice cream.' |
b'. | * | Jan gaat | het/dat. |
Jan goes | it/that |
Of course, one might try to solve this problem with modal verbs by assuming that example (70a') in fact contains a phonetically empty verb that corresponds to the semantically light verb doen'to do' in (71a), but this would leave unexplained why this verb cannot co-occur with the aspectual verb gaan.
a. | Jan wil | het/dat | doen. | |
Jan wants | it/that | do |
a'. | Jan wil | het/dat | ∅ | |
Jan wants | it/that | ∅ |
b. | Jan gaat | het/dat | doen. | |
Jan goes | it/that | do/∅ |
b'. | * | Jan gaat | het/dat | ∅. |
Jan goes | it/that | ∅ |
Furthermore, this line of thinking might lead us to expect the modal verb willen to exhibit the IPP-effect irrespective of whether the clause contains the verb doen or its phonetically empty counterpart ∅. The examples in (72) show that this expectation is not borne out: the effect does not occur when doen is not present.
a. | Jan heeft | het/dat | willen/*gewild | doen. | |
Jan has | it/that | want/wanted | do |
b. | Jan heeft | het/dat | gewild /*willen | ∅. | |
Jan has | it/that | wanted/want | ∅ |
Finally, the fact illustrated in (73) that modal verbs differ from aspectual verbs like gaan in that they can be combined with a nominal object is problematic for the view that the former is a non-main verb.
a. | Jan wil | een ijsje. | |
Jan wants | an ice.cream | ||
'Jan want to have an ice cream.' |
b. | * | Jan gaat | een ijsje. |
Jan goes | an ice.cream |
The examples above are intended to bring out that it is not a priori clear that the question as to whether or not a specific verb can be used as the only verb of a clause is cast iron proof for establishing whether or not that specific verb is a main verb. We will return to this issue in Section 4.6.
Semi-aspectual verbs correspond to main verbs like zitten'to sit', liggen'to lie', hangen'to hang' and staan'to stand' in (74), which refer to a certain posture or position of the subject of the clause, and certain verbs of movement like lopen'to walk'.
a. | Het boek staat | in de kast. | |
the book stands | in the bookcase | ||
'The book is in the bookcase.' |
b. | Het boek ligt | op tafel. | |
the book lies | on table | ||
'The book is lying on the table.' |
In their semi-aspectual use the lexical meaning of the main verb can but need not be present; examples like those in (75) can be used comfortably when the speaker cannot observe the referent of the subject of the clause and is thus not able to tell whether this referent is actually sitting or walking at the moment of speech. The primary function of the semi-aspectual verb is to indicate that we are dealing with an ongoing event and we are thus dealing with a progressive construction comparable to the English progressive construction: see the renderings given in (75).
a. | Jan zit | momenteel | te lezen. | |
Jan sits | at.present | to read | ||
'Jan is reading at the moment.' |
b. | Els loopt | momenteel | over het probleem | te piekeren. | |
Els walks | at.present | on that problem | to worry | ||
'Els is worrying about that problem at the moment.' |
The examples in (75) also show that semi-aspectual verbs differ from the modal and aspectual verbs in (68) in that they do not combine with bare infinitivals but with so-called te-infinitivals: leaving out the infinitival marker te leads to ungrammaticality. This is, however, not the case in the corresponding perfect-tense constructions in (76), in which the marker te can be dropped. The examples in (76) make it clear as well that the semi-aspectual verbs exhibit the IPP-effect; replacement of the infinitive zitten/lopen by the participle gezeten/gelopen leads to ungrammaticality.
a. | Jan heeft | de hele dag | zitten | (te) | lezen. | |
Jan has | the whole day | sit | to | read | ||
'Jan has been reading all day.' |
b. | Els heeft | de hele dag | over het probleem | lopen | (?te) | piekeren. | |
Els has | the whole day | on the problem | walk | to | worry | ||
'Els has been worrying about that problem all day.' |
The previous subsections have shown that non-main verbs impose certain restrictions on the morphological form of the main verb: temporal and passive auxiliaries select participles, modal/aspectual verbs select bare infinitivals, and (finite) semi-aspectual verbs select te-infinitivals. What we have not shown yet is that non-main verbs and the main verb they select obligatorily form a verbal complex, in which the main verb refers to some state of affairs and the non-main verbs function as modifiers providing supplementary information. This is clear from the fact that an embedded main verb normally cannot be the head of an independent finite clause introduced by the complementizer dat'that' or an infinitival clause introduced by the complementizer om. We illustrate this in (77) for the aspectual verb gaan and the semi-aspectual verb zitten; the number sign # is used to indicate that (77b) is possible if zitten is construed as a main verb and the infinitival clause is an adverbial purpose clause: "Jan sits in order to read the book".
a. | * | Jan gaat | dat | hij | het boek | leest. |
Jan goes | that | he | the book | reads |
b. | # | Jan zit | om | dat boek te lezen. |
Jan sits | comp | that book to read |
Observe that this test shows again that a modal verb like willen'to want' can be used as a main verb; see the discussion of (72) in Subsection II. We will return to the issue in Section 4.6.
a. | Jan wil | op tijd | komen. | |
Jan wants | in time | arrive | ||
'Jan wants to arrive there on time.' |
b. | Jan wil | dat | hij | op tijd | komt. | |
Jan want | that | he | in time | arrives | ||
'Jan wants that heʼll arrive there on time.' |
All examples in the subsections above have been presented as main with the non-main verb in second position. In most varieties of Dutch spoken in the Netherlands the auxiliaries cluster with the main verb in clause-final position; the arguments of the main verb must precede the non-main verb even when the main verb follows it. This clausal split is illustrated in (79) for the perfect auxiliary hebben'to have', the modal verb willen'to want' and the semi-aspectual verb zitten'to sit'.
a. | dat | Jan | <het boek> | heeft <*het boek> | gelezen. | |
that | Jan | the book | has | read | ||
'that Jan has read the book.' |
b. | dat | Jan | <het boek> | wil <%het boek> | lezen. | |
that | Jan | the book | wants | read | ||
'that Jan wants to read the book.' |
c. | dat | Jan | <het boek> | zit <%het boek> | te lezen. | |
that | Jan | the book | sits | to read | ||
'that Jan is reading the book.' |
We should note, however, that certain southern varieties of Dutch (including the standard variety spoken in Belgium) do allow the object to intervene between non-main verbs and (te-)infinitives, hence the use of the percentage sign in (79b&c). See Barbiers (2008: Section 2.3.1) and Chapter 7 for more detailed information.
The previous subsections have shown that auxiliaries must be accompanied by a main verb in the same clause. Furthermore, non-main verbs place restrictions on the form of the main verb they select: temporal and passive auxiliaries select participles, modal and aspectual verbs select bare infinitivals, and (finite) semi-aspectual verbs select te-infinitivals. Non-main verbs do not combine with clauses introduced by the complementizer dat or om, which strongly suggests that non-main verbs must form a single verbal complex with a main verb. Finally, we have seen that in the varieties of Dutch spoken in the Netherlands, clauses with non-main verbs exhibit monoclausal behavior in the sense that they trigger verb clustering, as a result of which the projection of the main verb must be split if the non-main verb is in clause-final position.
- 2008Syntactic atlas of the Dutch dialectsAmsterdamAmsterdam University Press