- Dutch1
- Frisian
- Saterfrisian
- Afrikaans
-
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological processes
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Word stress
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Monomorphemic words
- Diachronic aspects
- Generalizations on stress placement
- Default penultimate stress
- Lexical stress
- The closed penult restriction
- Final closed syllables
- The diphthong restriction
- Superheavy syllables (SHS)
- The three-syllable window
- Segmental restrictions
- Phonetic correlates
- Stress shifts in loanwords
- Quantity-sensitivity
- Secondary stress
- Vowel reduction in unstressed syllables
- Stress in complex words
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Accent & intonation
- Clitics
- Spelling
- Morphology
- Word formation
- Compounding
- Nominal compounds
- Verbal compounds
- Adjectival compounds
- Affixoids
- Coordinative compounds
- Synthetic compounds
- Reduplicative compounds
- Phrase-based compounds
- Elative compounds
- Exocentric compounds
- Linking elements
- Separable complex verbs (SCVs)
- Gapping of complex words
- Particle verbs
- Copulative compounds
- Derivation
- Numerals
- Derivation: inputs and input restrictions
- The meaning of affixes
- Non-native morphology
- Cohering and non-cohering affixes
- Prefixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixation: person nouns
- Conversion
- Pseudo-participles
- Bound forms
- Nouns
- Nominal prefixes
- Nominal suffixes
- -aal and -eel
- -aar
- -aard
- -aat
- -air
- -aris
- -ast
- Diminutives
- -dom
- -een
- -ees
- -el (nominal)
- -elaar
- -enis
- -er (nominal)
- -erd
- -erik
- -es
- -eur
- -euse
- ge...te
- -heid
- -iaan, -aan
- -ief
- -iek
- -ier
- -ier (French)
- -ière
- -iet
- -igheid
- -ij and allomorphs
- -ijn
- -in
- -ing
- -isme
- -ist
- -iteit
- -ling
- -oir
- -oot
- -rice
- -schap
- -schap (de)
- -schap (het)
- -sel
- -st
- -ster
- -t
- -tal
- -te
- -voud
- Verbs
- Adjectives
- Adverbs
- Univerbation
- Neo-classical word formation
- Construction-dependent morphology
- Morphological productivity
- Compounding
- Inflection
- Inflection and derivation
- Allomorphy
- The interface between phonology and morphology
- Word formation
- Syntax
- Preface and acknowledgements
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of verb phrases I:Argument structure
- 3 Projection of verb phrases II:Verb frame alternations
- Introduction
- 3.1. Main types
- 3.2. Alternations involving the external argument
- 3.3. Alternations of noun phrases and PPs
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.3.1.1. Dative alternation with aan-phrases (recipients)
- 3.3.1.2. Dative alternation with naar-phrases (goals)
- 3.3.1.3. Dative alternation with van-phrases (sources)
- 3.3.1.4. Dative alternation with bij-phrases (possessors)
- 3.3.1.5. Dative alternation with voor-phrases (benefactives)
- 3.3.1.6. Conclusion
- 3.3.1.7. Bibliographical notes
- 3.3.2. Accusative/PP alternations
- 3.3.3. Nominative/PP alternations
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.4. Some apparent cases of verb frame alternation
- 3.5. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of verb phrases IIIa:Selection of clauses/verb phrases
- 5 Projection of verb phrases IIIb:Argument and complementive clauses
- Introduction
- 5.1. Finite argument clauses
- 5.2. Infinitival argument clauses
- 5.3. Complementive clauses
- 6 Projection of verb phrases IIIc:Complements of non-main verbs
- 7 Projection of verb phrases IIId:Verb clusters
- 8 Projection of verb phrases IV: Adverbial modification
- 9 Word order in the clause I:General introduction
- 10 Word order in the clause II:Position of the finite verb (verb-first/second)
- 11 Word order in the clause III:Clause-initial position (wh-movement)
- Introduction
- 11.1. The formation of V1- and V2-clauses
- 11.2. Clause-initial position remains (phonetically) empty
- 11.3. Clause-initial position is filled
- 12 Word order in the clause IV:Postverbal field (extraposition)
- 13 Word order in the clause V: Middle field (scrambling)
- 14 Main-clause external elements
- Nouns and Noun Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of noun phrases I: complementation
- Introduction
- 2.1. General observations
- 2.2. Prepositional and nominal complements
- 2.3. Clausal complements
- 2.4. Bibliographical notes
- 3 Projection of noun phrases II: modification
- Introduction
- 3.1. Restrictive and non-restrictive modifiers
- 3.2. Premodification
- 3.3. Postmodification
- 3.3.1. Adpositional phrases
- 3.3.2. Relative clauses
- 3.3.3. Infinitival clauses
- 3.3.4. A special case: clauses referring to a proposition
- 3.3.5. Adjectival phrases
- 3.3.6. Adverbial postmodification
- 3.4. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of noun phrases III: binominal constructions
- Introduction
- 4.1. Binominal constructions without a preposition
- 4.2. Binominal constructions with a preposition
- 4.3. Bibliographical notes
- 5 Determiners: articles and pronouns
- Introduction
- 5.1. Articles
- 5.2. Pronouns
- 5.3. Bibliographical notes
- 6 Numerals and quantifiers
- 7 Pre-determiners
- Introduction
- 7.1. The universal quantifier al 'all' and its alternants
- 7.2. The pre-determiner heel 'all/whole'
- 7.3. A note on focus particles
- 7.4. Bibliographical notes
- 8 Syntactic uses of noun phrases
- Adjectives and Adjective Phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- 2 Projection of adjective phrases I: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adjective phrases II: Modification
- 4 Projection of adjective phrases III: Comparison
- 5 Attributive use of the adjective phrase
- 6 Predicative use of the adjective phrase
- 7 The partitive genitive construction
- 8 Adverbial use of the adjective phrase
- 9 Participles and infinitives: their adjectival use
- 10 Special constructions
- Adpositions and adpositional phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Introduction
- 1.1. Characterization of the category adposition
- 1.2. A formal classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3. A semantic classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3.1. Spatial adpositions
- 1.3.2. Temporal adpositions
- 1.3.3. Non-spatial/temporal prepositions
- 1.4. Borderline cases
- 1.5. Bibliographical notes
- 2 Projection of adpositional phrases: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adpositional phrases: Modification
- 4 Syntactic uses of the adpositional phrase
- 5 R-pronominalization and R-words
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Phonology
-
- General
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological Processes
- Assimilation
- Vowel nasalization
- Syllabic sonorants
- Final devoicing
- Fake geminates
- Vowel hiatus resolution
- Vowel reduction introduction
- Schwa deletion
- Schwa insertion
- /r/-deletion
- d-insertion
- {s/z}-insertion
- t-deletion
- Intrusive stop formation
- Breaking
- Vowel shortening
- h-deletion
- Replacement of the glide w
- Word stress
- Clitics
- Allomorphy
- Orthography of Frisian
- Morphology
- Inflection
- Word formation
- Derivation
- Prefixation
- Infixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixes
- Verbal suffixes
- Adjectival suffixes
- Adverbial suffixes
- Numeral suffixes
- Interjectional suffixes
- Onomastic suffixes
- Conversion
- Compositions
- Derivation
- Syntax
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Unergative and unaccusative subjects
- Evidentiality
- To-infinitival clauses
- Predication and noun incorporation
- Ellipsis
- Imperativus-pro-Infinitivo
- Expression of irrealis
- Embedded Verb Second
- Agreement
- Negation
- Nouns & Noun Phrases
- Classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Partitive noun constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Nominalised quantifiers
- Kind partitives
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Bare nominal attributions
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers and (pre)determiners
- Interrogative pronouns
- R-pronouns
- Syntactic uses
- Adjective Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification and degree quantification
- Comparison by degree
- Comparative
- Superlative
- Equative
- Attribution
- Agreement
- Attributive adjectives vs. prenominal elements
- Complex adjectives
- Noun ellipsis
- Co-occurring adjectives
- Predication
- Partitive adjective constructions
- Adverbial use
- Participles and infinitives
- Adposition Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Intransitive adpositions
- Predication
- Preposition stranding
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
-
- General
- Morphology
- Morphology
- 1 Word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 1.1.1 Compounds and their heads
- 1.1.2 Special types of compounds
- 1.1.2.1 Affixoids
- 1.1.2.2 Coordinative compounds
- 1.1.2.3 Synthetic compounds and complex pseudo-participles
- 1.1.2.4 Reduplicative compounds
- 1.1.2.5 Phrase-based compounds
- 1.1.2.6 Elative compounds
- 1.1.2.7 Exocentric compounds
- 1.1.2.8 Linking elements
- 1.1.2.9 Separable Complex Verbs and Particle Verbs
- 1.1.2.10 Noun Incorporation Verbs
- 1.1.2.11 Gapping
- 1.2 Derivation
- 1.3 Minor patterns of word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 2 Inflection
- 1 Word formation
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
- 0 Introduction to the AP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of APs
- 2 Complementation of APs
- 3 Modification and degree quantification of APs
- 4 Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative
- 5 Attribution of APs
- 6 Predication of APs
- 7 The partitive adjective construction
- 8 Adverbial use of APs
- 9 Participles and infinitives as APs
- Nouns and Noun Phrases (NPs)
- 0 Introduction to the NP
- 1 Characteristics and Classification of NPs
- 2 Complementation of NPs
- 3 Modification of NPs
- 3.1 Modification of NP by Determiners and APs
- 3.2 Modification of NP by PP
- 3.3 Modification of NP by adverbial clauses
- 3.4 Modification of NP by possessors
- 3.5 Modification of NP by relative clauses
- 3.6 Modification of NP in a cleft construction
- 3.7 Free relative clauses and selected interrogative clauses
- 4 Partitive noun constructions and constructions related to them
- 4.1 The referential partitive construction
- 4.2 The partitive construction of abstract quantity
- 4.3 The numerical partitive construction
- 4.4 The partitive interrogative construction
- 4.5 Adjectival, nominal and nominalised partitive quantifiers
- 4.6 Kind partitives
- 4.7 Partitive predication with a preposition
- 4.8 Bare nominal attribution
- 5 Articles and names
- 6 Pronouns
- 7 Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- 8 Interrogative pronouns
- 9 R-pronouns and the indefinite expletive
- 10 Syntactic functions of Noun Phrases
- Adpositions and Adpositional Phrases (PPs)
- 0 Introduction to the PP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of PPs
- 2 Complementation of PPs
- 3 Modification of PPs
- 4 Bare (intransitive) adpositions
- 5 Predication of PPs
- 6 Form and distribution of adpositions with respect to staticity and construction type
- 7 Adpositional complements and adverbials
- Verbs and Verb Phrases (VPs)
- 0 Introduction to the VP in Saterland Frisian
- 1 Characteristics and classification of verbs
- 2 Unergative and unaccusative subjects and the auxiliary of the perfect
- 3 Evidentiality in relation to perception and epistemicity
- 4 Types of to-infinitival constituents
- 5 Predication
- 5.1 The auxiliary of being and its selection restrictions
- 5.2 The auxiliary of going and its selection restrictions
- 5.3 The auxiliary of continuation and its selection restrictions
- 5.4 The auxiliary of coming and its selection restrictions
- 5.5 Modal auxiliaries and their selection restrictions
- 5.6 Auxiliaries of body posture and aspect and their selection restrictions
- 5.7 Transitive verbs of predication
- 5.8 The auxiliary of doing used as a semantically empty finite auxiliary
- 5.9 Supplementive predication
- 6 The verbal paradigm, irregularity and suppletion
- 7 Verb Second and the word order in main and embedded clauses
- 8 Various aspects of clause structure
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
-
- General
- Phonology
- Afrikaans phonology
- Segment inventory
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- The diphthongised long vowels /e/, /ø/ and /o/
- The unrounded mid-front vowel /ɛ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /ɑ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /a/
- The rounded mid-high back vowel /ɔ/
- The rounded high back vowel /u/
- The rounded and unrounded high front vowels /i/ and /y/
- The unrounded and rounded central vowels /ə/ and /œ/
- The diphthongs /əi/, /œy/ and /œu/
- Overview of Afrikaans consonants
- The bilabial plosives /p/ and /b/
- The alveolar plosives /t/ and /d/
- The velar plosives /k/ and /g/
- The bilabial nasal /m/
- The alveolar nasal /n/
- The velar nasal /ŋ/
- The trill /r/
- The lateral liquid /l/
- The alveolar fricative /s/
- The velar fricative /x/
- The labiodental fricatives /f/ and /v/
- The approximants /ɦ/, /j/ and /ʋ/
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- Word stress
- The phonetic properties of stress
- Primary stress on monomorphemic words in Afrikaans
- Background to primary stress in monomorphemes in Afrikaans
- Overview of the Main Stress Rule of Afrikaans
- The short vowels of Afrikaans
- Long vowels in monomorphemes
- Primary stress on diphthongs in monomorphemes
- Exceptions
- Stress shifts in place names
- Stress shift towards word-final position
- Stress pattern of reduplications
- Phonological processes
- Vowel related processes
- Consonant related processes
- Homorganic glide insertion
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Phonotactics
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Afrikaans syntax
- Nouns and noun phrases
- Characteristics of the NP
- Classification of nouns
- Complementation of NPs
- Modification of NPs
- Binominal and partitive constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Partitive constructions with nominalised quantifiers
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Binominal name constructions
- Binominal genitive constructions
- Bare nominal attribution
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- Syntactic uses of the noun phrase
- Adjectives and adjective phrases
- Characteristics and classification of the AP
- Complementation of APs
- Modification and Degree Quantification of APs
- Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative degree
- Attribution of APs
- Predication of APs
- The partitive adjective construction
- Adverbial use of APs
- Participles and infinitives as adjectives
- Verbs and verb phrases
- Characterisation and classification
- Argument structure
- Verb frame alternations
- Complements of non-main verbs
- Verb clusters
- Complement clauses
- Adverbial modification
- Word order in the clause: Introduction
- Word order in the clause: position of the finite Verb
- Word order in the clause: Clause-initial position
- Word order in the clause: Extraposition and right-dislocation in the postverbal field
- Word order in the middle field
- Emphatic constructions
- Adpositions and adposition phrases
-ing [Iŋ] is an unstressed, productive, cohering Germanic suffix found in nouns of common gender derived from verbs. -ing formations typically are names of the activity denoted by the base verb, or the result of that activity. Input verbs may be simplex or complex, native or non-native, with a certain preference for complex ones. Being of common gender, -ing derivations take the singular definite article de. Plural of -ing formations is in -en. A few -ing formations have (only or also) a more concrete meaning.
- the activity denoted by V
- the result of the activity denoted by V
uitvinding invention | < uitvinden to invent |
wending turn | < wenden to turn |
verhoging increase, platform | < verhogen to increase |
The suffix -ing [Iŋ] is a nominalizing Germanic suffix. It attaches mainly to transitive bases, but occasionally one also finds ergative inputs, as in ontploffing explosion < ontploffen to explode, or unergative ones, as in neiging tendency < neigen to tend(De Haas and Trommelen 1993: 243). Nouns with the suffix -ing are of common gender, selecting the singular definite article de.
Booij (2002: 37-8) and others have noted that all nouns ending in –ing are of common gender, whether they are simplex or complex nouns. This applies, for instance, to the following words: 'haring herring, 'koning king, 'paling eel, ring ring, se'ring lilac. The only exception is the noun ding thing, which is neuter. This state of affairs has brought some linguists (e.g. Trommelen (1984)) to assume a suffix –ing in all polysyllabic nouns in -ing with penultimate stress, in parallel to the deverbal suffix -ing: both deverbal -ing and the alleged suffix always create de- nouns, and always select the diminutive allomorph -etje. According to Booij, this cannot be accepted as an adequate analysis, since there is no independent evidence for the suffix status of the ending –ing in these nouns. Instead, the phonological form of nouns may play a role in gender assignment.
Historically, there is ground in assuming noun-forming suffixes -ing that took non-verbal bases, e.g. in fish names like paling eel, wijting whiting, bokking smoked herring (also found in reduced plus assimilated form as bokkem /'bɔ-kəm/), houting houting, whitefish, spiering smelt, etc., in object names like heining fence, schutting fence and zoldering loft, ceiling and in person's names like edeling member of the nobility < edel noble (A) and hemeling someone from heavens < hemel heaven(Instituut voor Nederlandse Lexicologie 1995). These suffixes are no longer productive.
-ing attaches productively to verbs denoting a process or an achievement, both native and nonnative, simplex or complex. It is particularly productive for complex verbs; for simplex verbs, ing- derivation is either blocked by the existence of a nominalisation derived in other ways (e.g., conversion or the infinitival form), or there is a semantic or other difference between the two nominalisations (Van Haeringen 1971), (Booij 2002: 125):
Base verb | Noun derived with -ing | Noun otherwise derived |
beleven to live to see, to experience | beleving experiencing | belevenis experience |
bestrijden to fight | bestrijding fight against | *bestrijd |
betogen to argue, to demonstrate | betoging (public) demonstration, rally | betoog argument, speech |
bidden to pray | *bidding | gebed prayer |
aanbidden to worship | aanbidding worship | *aangebed |
komen to come | *koming | komst coming |
ontkomen to escape | ontkoming escape | *ontkomst |
roepen to call | roeping vocation | roep call |
spelen to play | speling freak of nature | spel play |
strijden to fight | *strijding | strijd fight |
bestrijden to fight, to oppose | bestrijding fight, control | *bestrijd |
demonstreren to demonstrate | *demonstrering | demonstratie demonstration |
reserveren to reservate | reservering reservation | reservatie reservation |
Both reservering reservation and reservatie reservation are attested, but the latter is much less frequent, and is found mainly in Belgium.
Various verbs occur only with -ing in synthetic compounds, e.g. *geving < geven to give is impossible, but zingeving sense giving legitimation, justification is fine (just like ingeving idea which is derived from the particle verb ingeven). Another case in point is komen to come: *koming does not exist, but we do find nouns like totstandkoming to stand coming realization, tegemoetkoming advance, concession, allowance (< scvtegemoetkomen to meet, to accomodate) and tekortkoming shortcoming, failure. The same is found in the few verbs with a monosyllabic infinitive that can be suffixed with -ing, and then only if the verb is either prefixed, or in a synthetic compound: bedoening affair (< *bedoen?), genoegdoening satisfaction, *doening (< doen to do); herziening revision, but no *ziening (< zien to see).
The suffix -ing appears to have a preference for transitive verbs.
De Haas and Trommelen (1993) suggest that it is no coincidence that -ing prefers complex verbs, noting that verbal prefixation often involves transitivization: according to them, a verb like drijven to drive, to conduct is unergative which explains the impossibility of *drijving. On the other hand, prefixed forms like verdrijven to expel and aandrijven to drive on, to propel are transitive, and the corresponding -ing formations verdrijving expulsion and aandrijving propulsion, instigation are fine.
The restriction of -ing to transitive bases is not absolute, as shown by the existence of forms like ontploffing explosion (< ergative ontploffen to explode) and neiging tendency (< unergative neigen to tend). De Haas and Trommelen (1993) note that it is hard to interpret these forms as nomina actionis.
The suffix -ing [Iŋ] is a Germanic suffix that does not carry main stress, nor does it change the stress pattern of the word it attaches to: ont'hechten to detach > onthechting /ɔnt-'hɛχ-tɪŋ/ detachment, 'ademhalen to breathe > ademhaling /'a-dəm-ha-lɪŋ/ breath; the suffix may carry secondary stress, witness the selection of the -etje diminutive allomorph in words like wandelingetje small stroll (< wandelen to walk). It is a cohering suffix: syllabification does not respect the morphological boundary, as shown in the examples just given.
The plural form of -ing formations, if applicable, is in –en: uitvindingen inventions, kapingen hijackings, pogingen efforts, bekeringen conversions.
Loans from English in -ing usually have plural forms in -s: dancings piercings briefings leggings parkings healings mailings shillings etc. These data shows once again that foreign words can be borrowed with plural forms - cf. Latin such as loans museum, plural form musea (next to regular musea) and crisis, plural form crises (next to regular crisissen).
Formations with the suffix -ing denote primarily actions and processes, but systematically also the result of these actions/processes.
In addition, -ing derivations may also denote objects (e.g. storting deposit < storten to shed, to dump, to deposit ), means (voeding food < voeden to feed), locations (woning house < wonen to live), or collective agents (e.g. beweging popular movement < bewegen to move).
According to Booij (2002: 106) this is a pattern found across different morphological classes, as illustrated by the following examples (all these morphological types have a primary action interpretation):
- result: gedicht poem (< dichten to make poetry), schilder-ij painting (< schilderen to paint)
- object: bijsluiter package insert (< bijsluiten to enclose), verzin-sel fiction (< verzinnen to invent)
- means: smeersel ointment (< smeren to smear, to rub), verzacht-er softener (< verzachten to soften)
- location: gebouw building (< bouwen to build), optrekje cottage (< optrekken to raise)
- collective: gehoor audience (horen to hear)
-ing derivations may inherit argument structure and subcategorization properties of the base verb. In derivations of transitive base verbs, the direct object may be realized as the lefthand part of a compound or as a prepositional phrase with van whereas the Agent role can be expressed by means of a prepositional phrase headed by door: afvalscheiding door bedrijven waste separation by companies, de scheiding van afval door bedrijven the separation of waste by companies (see also Broekhuis et al. on inheritance of argument structure and subcategorization properties).
Booij (2002: 194 vv) notes that if a thematic role is not expressed, the relevant entity that bears this role will be reconstructed on the basis of knowledge of the context and situation, and knowledge of the world. In this respect nouns differ systematically from verbs because normally, the thematic roles of a verb must be expressed as indicated in their Predicated Argument Structure. For instance, since the verb verwoesten to destroy is obligatorily transitive, both roles must be expressed, and a sentence like *De vijand verwoest The enemy destroys is ungrammatical, whereas NPs with the deverbal noun verwoesting destruction as their head do not need to express the Patient-role if it is clear which entity has that role: De verwoesting door de vijand duurde weken The destruction by the enemy went on for weeks.
These observations suggest that the LCS-roles of a verb are semantically still active in the corresponding deverbal noun, but can also be identified in other ways than by means of overt syntactic expression. Both Hoeksema (1984) and Mackenzie (1985) express this generalization in terms of a rule of optional argument reduction for deverbal nouns. The deverbal nouns then still differ from simplex ones in that the latter do not have specific semantic roles associated with them. For instance, the use of a door-PP requires the presence at LCS of an Agent-role, and hence there is difference in wellformedness between the ungrammatical *het boek door Jelle the book by Jelle and the grammatical de publicatie door Jelle the publication by Jelle: the latter allows for an event interpretation with an Agent. Note, however, that event nouns can also acquire a result meaning; in the latter case, the event reading and the associated thematic roles are no longer available, as shown by the contrast between De publicatie door Jelle was een moedige stap The publication by Jelle was a courageous step and *Die gele publicatie door Jelle was belangrijk That yellow publication by Jelle was important. In the second example, the use of the colour adjective geel yellow implies a result interpretation of publicatie. Hence, the Agent role that comes with the event interpretation is not available for being linked to the door-PP.
The suffix –ing competes with the prefix ge-, in particular for verbs belonging to the informal register, with the suffix -erij (cf. Hüning (1999)Hüning (1992), and with -sel for the result reading. The suffix -ing also competes with -atie in the domains of verbs in -eren. However, in this case both forms are often possible, without a difference in meaning, as in redenering - redenatie reasoning, both derived from the verb redeneren to reason. Sometimes, the noun in -atie is more common in southern Dutch, whereas the north prefers the formation in -ering. A case in point is reservatie reservation versus reservering reservation, both derived from the verb reserveren to reserve. Both forms are attested in both parts of the Dutch language area, but the former variant is more frequent in southern Dutch, whereas the latter is preferred in northern Dutch.
-ing derivations are input to regular diminutive formation in -kje or -etje, depending on the stress pattern of the base: if the last syllable (in -ing) carries secondary stress, the allomorph -etje appears (verzekeringetje insurance-DIM 'insignificant insurance), if not, we find -kje (overtredinkje small offense). Further suffixation is rare, although we do find occasional adjectives with suffixes like -loos (e.g. bewegingsloos motionless) and -achtig (klantenserviceafdelingachtig customer support section like).
The restriction on further suffixation of -ing formations is not caused by the phonological make-up of the suffix, as comparison with the suffix -ling makes clear: formations with -ling can be input to new derivations, e.g. with the nominalizing suffix -schap, as in nakomelingschap after-come-er-ship offspring.
Formations with -ing easily enter into NN-compounds. If they function as lefthand members, they often get a linking element /s/ (verwachtingshorizon expect.ing.s.horizon horizon of expectation, verzekeringsagent insure.ing.s.agent insurance agent). If the -ing formation is the righthand part, the lefthand part gets an argument reading, if posssible: noodverordening emergency ordinance, reisverzekering travel insurance, voedselverspilling food wastage, stroomstoring power failure.
- 2002The morphology of DutchOxfordOxford University Press
- 2002The morphology of DutchOxfordOxford University Press
- 2002The morphology of DutchOxfordOxford University Press
- 2002The morphology of DutchOxfordOxford University Press
- 1993Morfologisch handboek van het Nederlands. Een overzicht van de woordvormingSDU Uitgeverij
- 1993Morfologisch handboek van het Nederlands. Een overzicht van de woordvormingSDU Uitgeverij
- 1993Morfologisch handboek van het Nederlands. Een overzicht van de woordvormingSDU Uitgeverij
- 1971Het achtervoegsel -ing, mogelijkheden en beperkingenDe Nieuwe Taalgids64449-68
- 1984Categorial morphologyGroningenPh. D. dissertation, University of Groningen
- 1992De concurrentie tussen deverbale nomina met <i>ge</i>- en op -<i>erij</i>Spektator21161-172
- 1999Woordensmederij. De geschiedenis van het suffix -erijUtrechtLOT
- 1995Het Woordenboek der Nederlandsche Taal (WNT)
- 1985GenominaliseerValentie in Functionele Grammatica. Interdisciplinair Tijdschrift voor Taal- en Tekstwetenschap5177-198
- 1984The Syllable in DutchDordrechtForis