- Dutch1
- Frisian
- Saterfrisian
- Afrikaans
-
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological processes
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Word stress
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Monomorphemic words
- Diachronic aspects
- Generalizations on stress placement
- Default penultimate stress
- Lexical stress
- The closed penult restriction
- Final closed syllables
- The diphthong restriction
- Superheavy syllables (SHS)
- The three-syllable window
- Segmental restrictions
- Phonetic correlates
- Stress shifts in loanwords
- Quantity-sensitivity
- Secondary stress
- Vowel reduction in unstressed syllables
- Stress in complex words
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Accent & intonation
- Clitics
- Spelling
- Morphology
- Word formation
- Compounding
- Nominal compounds
- Verbal compounds
- Adjectival compounds
- Affixoids
- Coordinative compounds
- Synthetic compounds
- Reduplicative compounds
- Phrase-based compounds
- Elative compounds
- Exocentric compounds
- Linking elements
- Separable complex verbs (SCVs)
- Gapping of complex words
- Particle verbs
- Copulative compounds
- Derivation
- Numerals
- Derivation: inputs and input restrictions
- The meaning of affixes
- Non-native morphology
- Cohering and non-cohering affixes
- Prefixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixation: person nouns
- Conversion
- Pseudo-participles
- Bound forms
- Nouns
- Nominal prefixes
- Nominal suffixes
- -aal and -eel
- -aar
- -aard
- -aat
- -air
- -aris
- -ast
- Diminutives
- -dom
- -een
- -ees
- -el (nominal)
- -elaar
- -enis
- -er (nominal)
- -erd
- -erik
- -es
- -eur
- -euse
- ge...te
- -heid
- -iaan, -aan
- -ief
- -iek
- -ier
- -ier (French)
- -ière
- -iet
- -igheid
- -ij and allomorphs
- -ijn
- -in
- -ing
- -isme
- -ist
- -iteit
- -ling
- -oir
- -oot
- -rice
- -schap
- -schap (de)
- -schap (het)
- -sel
- -st
- -ster
- -t
- -tal
- -te
- -voud
- Verbs
- Adjectives
- Adverbs
- Univerbation
- Neo-classical word formation
- Construction-dependent morphology
- Morphological productivity
- Compounding
- Inflection
- Inflection and derivation
- Allomorphy
- The interface between phonology and morphology
- Word formation
- Syntax
- Preface and acknowledgements
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of verb phrases I:Argument structure
- 3 Projection of verb phrases II:Verb frame alternations
- Introduction
- 3.1. Main types
- 3.2. Alternations involving the external argument
- 3.3. Alternations of noun phrases and PPs
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.3.1.1. Dative alternation with aan-phrases (recipients)
- 3.3.1.2. Dative alternation with naar-phrases (goals)
- 3.3.1.3. Dative alternation with van-phrases (sources)
- 3.3.1.4. Dative alternation with bij-phrases (possessors)
- 3.3.1.5. Dative alternation with voor-phrases (benefactives)
- 3.3.1.6. Conclusion
- 3.3.1.7. Bibliographical notes
- 3.3.2. Accusative/PP alternations
- 3.3.3. Nominative/PP alternations
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.4. Some apparent cases of verb frame alternation
- 3.5. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of verb phrases IIIa:Selection of clauses/verb phrases
- 5 Projection of verb phrases IIIb:Argument and complementive clauses
- Introduction
- 5.1. Finite argument clauses
- 5.2. Infinitival argument clauses
- 5.3. Complementive clauses
- 6 Projection of verb phrases IIIc:Complements of non-main verbs
- 7 Projection of verb phrases IIId:Verb clusters
- 8 Projection of verb phrases IV: Adverbial modification
- 9 Word order in the clause I:General introduction
- 10 Word order in the clause II:Position of the finite verb (verb-first/second)
- 11 Word order in the clause III:Clause-initial position (wh-movement)
- Introduction
- 11.1. The formation of V1- and V2-clauses
- 11.2. Clause-initial position remains (phonetically) empty
- 11.3. Clause-initial position is filled
- 12 Word order in the clause IV:Postverbal field (extraposition)
- 13 Word order in the clause V: Middle field (scrambling)
- 14 Main-clause external elements
- Nouns and Noun Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of noun phrases I: complementation
- Introduction
- 2.1. General observations
- 2.2. Prepositional and nominal complements
- 2.3. Clausal complements
- 2.4. Bibliographical notes
- 3 Projection of noun phrases II: modification
- Introduction
- 3.1. Restrictive and non-restrictive modifiers
- 3.2. Premodification
- 3.3. Postmodification
- 3.3.1. Adpositional phrases
- 3.3.2. Relative clauses
- 3.3.3. Infinitival clauses
- 3.3.4. A special case: clauses referring to a proposition
- 3.3.5. Adjectival phrases
- 3.3.6. Adverbial postmodification
- 3.4. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of noun phrases III: binominal constructions
- Introduction
- 4.1. Binominal constructions without a preposition
- 4.2. Binominal constructions with a preposition
- 4.3. Bibliographical notes
- 5 Determiners: articles and pronouns
- Introduction
- 5.1. Articles
- 5.2. Pronouns
- 5.3. Bibliographical notes
- 6 Numerals and quantifiers
- 7 Pre-determiners
- Introduction
- 7.1. The universal quantifier al 'all' and its alternants
- 7.2. The pre-determiner heel 'all/whole'
- 7.3. A note on focus particles
- 7.4. Bibliographical notes
- 8 Syntactic uses of noun phrases
- Adjectives and Adjective Phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- 2 Projection of adjective phrases I: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adjective phrases II: Modification
- 4 Projection of adjective phrases III: Comparison
- 5 Attributive use of the adjective phrase
- 6 Predicative use of the adjective phrase
- 7 The partitive genitive construction
- 8 Adverbial use of the adjective phrase
- 9 Participles and infinitives: their adjectival use
- 10 Special constructions
- Adpositions and adpositional phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Introduction
- 1.1. Characterization of the category adposition
- 1.2. A formal classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3. A semantic classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3.1. Spatial adpositions
- 1.3.2. Temporal adpositions
- 1.3.3. Non-spatial/temporal prepositions
- 1.4. Borderline cases
- 1.5. Bibliographical notes
- 2 Projection of adpositional phrases: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adpositional phrases: Modification
- 4 Syntactic uses of the adpositional phrase
- 5 R-pronominalization and R-words
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Phonology
-
- General
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological Processes
- Assimilation
- Vowel nasalization
- Syllabic sonorants
- Final devoicing
- Fake geminates
- Vowel hiatus resolution
- Vowel reduction introduction
- Schwa deletion
- Schwa insertion
- /r/-deletion
- d-insertion
- {s/z}-insertion
- t-deletion
- Intrusive stop formation
- Breaking
- Vowel shortening
- h-deletion
- Replacement of the glide w
- Word stress
- Clitics
- Allomorphy
- Orthography of Frisian
- Morphology
- Inflection
- Word formation
- Derivation
- Prefixation
- Infixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixes
- Verbal suffixes
- Adjectival suffixes
- Adverbial suffixes
- Numeral suffixes
- Interjectional suffixes
- Onomastic suffixes
- Conversion
- Compositions
- Derivation
- Syntax
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Unergative and unaccusative subjects
- Evidentiality
- To-infinitival clauses
- Predication and noun incorporation
- Ellipsis
- Imperativus-pro-Infinitivo
- Expression of irrealis
- Embedded Verb Second
- Agreement
- Negation
- Nouns & Noun Phrases
- Classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Partitive noun constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Nominalised quantifiers
- Kind partitives
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Bare nominal attributions
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers and (pre)determiners
- Interrogative pronouns
- R-pronouns
- Syntactic uses
- Adjective Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification and degree quantification
- Comparison by degree
- Comparative
- Superlative
- Equative
- Attribution
- Agreement
- Attributive adjectives vs. prenominal elements
- Complex adjectives
- Noun ellipsis
- Co-occurring adjectives
- Predication
- Partitive adjective constructions
- Adverbial use
- Participles and infinitives
- Adposition Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Intransitive adpositions
- Predication
- Preposition stranding
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
-
- General
- Morphology
- Morphology
- 1 Word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 1.1.1 Compounds and their heads
- 1.1.2 Special types of compounds
- 1.1.2.1 Affixoids
- 1.1.2.2 Coordinative compounds
- 1.1.2.3 Synthetic compounds and complex pseudo-participles
- 1.1.2.4 Reduplicative compounds
- 1.1.2.5 Phrase-based compounds
- 1.1.2.6 Elative compounds
- 1.1.2.7 Exocentric compounds
- 1.1.2.8 Linking elements
- 1.1.2.9 Separable Complex Verbs and Particle Verbs
- 1.1.2.10 Noun Incorporation Verbs
- 1.1.2.11 Gapping
- 1.2 Derivation
- 1.3 Minor patterns of word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 2 Inflection
- 1 Word formation
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
- 0 Introduction to the AP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of APs
- 2 Complementation of APs
- 3 Modification and degree quantification of APs
- 4 Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative
- 5 Attribution of APs
- 6 Predication of APs
- 7 The partitive adjective construction
- 8 Adverbial use of APs
- 9 Participles and infinitives as APs
- Nouns and Noun Phrases (NPs)
- 0 Introduction to the NP
- 1 Characteristics and Classification of NPs
- 2 Complementation of NPs
- 3 Modification of NPs
- 3.1 Modification of NP by Determiners and APs
- 3.2 Modification of NP by PP
- 3.3 Modification of NP by adverbial clauses
- 3.4 Modification of NP by possessors
- 3.5 Modification of NP by relative clauses
- 3.6 Modification of NP in a cleft construction
- 3.7 Free relative clauses and selected interrogative clauses
- 4 Partitive noun constructions and constructions related to them
- 4.1 The referential partitive construction
- 4.2 The partitive construction of abstract quantity
- 4.3 The numerical partitive construction
- 4.4 The partitive interrogative construction
- 4.5 Adjectival, nominal and nominalised partitive quantifiers
- 4.6 Kind partitives
- 4.7 Partitive predication with a preposition
- 4.8 Bare nominal attribution
- 5 Articles and names
- 6 Pronouns
- 7 Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- 8 Interrogative pronouns
- 9 R-pronouns and the indefinite expletive
- 10 Syntactic functions of Noun Phrases
- Adpositions and Adpositional Phrases (PPs)
- 0 Introduction to the PP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of PPs
- 2 Complementation of PPs
- 3 Modification of PPs
- 4 Bare (intransitive) adpositions
- 5 Predication of PPs
- 6 Form and distribution of adpositions with respect to staticity and construction type
- 7 Adpositional complements and adverbials
- Verbs and Verb Phrases (VPs)
- 0 Introduction to the VP in Saterland Frisian
- 1 Characteristics and classification of verbs
- 2 Unergative and unaccusative subjects and the auxiliary of the perfect
- 3 Evidentiality in relation to perception and epistemicity
- 4 Types of to-infinitival constituents
- 5 Predication
- 5.1 The auxiliary of being and its selection restrictions
- 5.2 The auxiliary of going and its selection restrictions
- 5.3 The auxiliary of continuation and its selection restrictions
- 5.4 The auxiliary of coming and its selection restrictions
- 5.5 Modal auxiliaries and their selection restrictions
- 5.6 Auxiliaries of body posture and aspect and their selection restrictions
- 5.7 Transitive verbs of predication
- 5.8 The auxiliary of doing used as a semantically empty finite auxiliary
- 5.9 Supplementive predication
- 6 The verbal paradigm, irregularity and suppletion
- 7 Verb Second and the word order in main and embedded clauses
- 8 Various aspects of clause structure
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
-
- General
- Phonology
- Afrikaans phonology
- Segment inventory
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- The diphthongised long vowels /e/, /ø/ and /o/
- The unrounded mid-front vowel /ɛ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /ɑ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /a/
- The rounded mid-high back vowel /ɔ/
- The rounded high back vowel /u/
- The rounded and unrounded high front vowels /i/ and /y/
- The unrounded and rounded central vowels /ə/ and /œ/
- The diphthongs /əi/, /œy/ and /œu/
- Overview of Afrikaans consonants
- The bilabial plosives /p/ and /b/
- The alveolar plosives /t/ and /d/
- The velar plosives /k/ and /g/
- The bilabial nasal /m/
- The alveolar nasal /n/
- The velar nasal /ŋ/
- The trill /r/
- The lateral liquid /l/
- The alveolar fricative /s/
- The velar fricative /x/
- The labiodental fricatives /f/ and /v/
- The approximants /ɦ/, /j/ and /ʋ/
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- Word stress
- The phonetic properties of stress
- Primary stress on monomorphemic words in Afrikaans
- Background to primary stress in monomorphemes in Afrikaans
- Overview of the Main Stress Rule of Afrikaans
- The short vowels of Afrikaans
- Long vowels in monomorphemes
- Primary stress on diphthongs in monomorphemes
- Exceptions
- Stress shifts in place names
- Stress shift towards word-final position
- Stress pattern of reduplications
- Phonological processes
- Vowel related processes
- Consonant related processes
- Homorganic glide insertion
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Phonotactics
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Afrikaans syntax
- Nouns and noun phrases
- Characteristics of the NP
- Classification of nouns
- Complementation of NPs
- Modification of NPs
- Binominal and partitive constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Partitive constructions with nominalised quantifiers
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Binominal name constructions
- Binominal genitive constructions
- Bare nominal attribution
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- Syntactic uses of the noun phrase
- Adjectives and adjective phrases
- Characteristics and classification of the AP
- Complementation of APs
- Modification and Degree Quantification of APs
- Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative degree
- Attribution of APs
- Predication of APs
- The partitive adjective construction
- Adverbial use of APs
- Participles and infinitives as adjectives
- Verbs and verb phrases
- Characterisation and classification
- Argument structure
- Verb frame alternations
- Complements of non-main verbs
- Verb clusters
- Complement clauses
- Adverbial modification
- Word order in the clause: Introduction
- Word order in the clause: position of the finite Verb
- Word order in the clause: Clause-initial position
- Word order in the clause: Extraposition and right-dislocation in the postverbal field
- Word order in the middle field
- Emphatic constructions
- Adpositions and adposition phrases
This section discusses the adjectival part of the partitive genitive construction. We will focus especially on the question of what adjective types can be used in this construction. Anticipating what follows, we can say that the correct generalization seems to be that the adjectives that occur in the partitive genitive construction constitute a proper subset of the adjectives that can be used as complementives: in other words, adjectives that can be used attributively only are excluded from this construction.
Adjectival part of the partitive genitive construction: the set of partitive genitive adjectives is a proper subset of the adjectives that can be used as complementives. |
Section 1.3 has distinguished four semantic classes of adjectives: (i) the set-denoting, (ii) the relational, and (iii) the evaluative adjectives, and what we have called (iv) the residue. It has been shown there that all adjectives in class (i), a restricted set of adjectives from class (ii), and virtually none of the adjectives in classes (iii) and (iv) can be used as complementives. This section will show that these findings correspond nicely with what we find in the partitive genitive construction.
Generally speaking, set-denoting adjectives can be readily used both in prenominal attributive position and as complementives; cf. Section 1.3.2. The doubly-primed examples in (55) show that these adjectives also occur as partitive genitives.
a. | een handige doek | |
a handy towel |
a'. | Deze doek is handig. | |
this towel is handy |
a''. | iets handigs | |
something handy |
b. | een klein doosje | |
a small box |
b'. | Het doosje is klein. | |
the box is small |
b''. | iets kleins | |
something small |
c. | een speciale kleur | |
a special color |
c'. | Die kleur is speciaal. | |
that color is special |
c''. | iets speciaals | |
something special |
This does not mean, however, that all set-denoting adjectives can be used in the partitive genitive construction; the subsections below will show that the six subclasses of set-denoting adjectives in (56) cannot.
a. | adjectives that can only be predicated of +animate noun phrases; |
b. | adjectives that take a proposition as their logical subject; |
c. | adjectives that take weather het as their subject; |
d. | adjectives that take a nominal complement; |
e. | superlatives; |
f. | adjectives that end in /a/, /o/, /i/, /e/, /y/ or schwa. |
The first subclass consists of adjectives that, at least in their predicative use, express properties that can only be attributed to a +animate entity. Some examples are given in (57) to (59); see Section 1.3.4 for a discussion of the examples in (57b) and (58b).
a. | een dronken man | |
a drunk man |
a'. | Die man is dronken. | |
that men is drunk |
a''. | * | iets dronkens |
something drunk |
b. | een dronken bui | |
a drunken fit |
b'. | * | Die bui is dronken. |
that fit is drunk |
a. | een verlegen jongen | |
a shy boy |
a'. | De jongen is verlegen. | |
the boy is shy |
a''. | * | iets verlegens |
something shy |
b. | een verlegen glimlach | |
a shy smile |
b'. | * | Die glimlach is verlegen. |
that smile is shy |
a. | een zwangere vrouw | |
a pregnant woman |
a'. | De vrouw is zwanger. | |
the woman is pregnant |
a''. | * | iets zwangers |
something pregnant |
b. | een loopse teef | |
an in.season bitch |
b'. | Deze teef is loops. | |
this bitch is in.season |
b''. | * | iets loops |
something in.season |
Subsection F will show that adjectives that end in a schwa, such as beige'beige' or frêle'delicate', give rise to a marked result in the partitive genitive construction: ?iets beiges/frêles. One might therefore want to claim that the doubly-primed examples in (57) and (58) are excluded because the adjectives dronken and verlegen are normally pronounced with a final schwa. However, other cases of adjectives ending in en do occasionally occur in this construction, in which case the /n/ seems to be phonetically realized; cf. (68b) below. In fact, the relevant examples are judged acceptable by some (but not all) speakers in contexts like (60a&b), which show that they are not blocked for phonological reasons. The cases in (60) are special in that the adjectives do not attribute a property to an animate being: iets dronkens in (60a) refers to some aspect of Janʼs appearance, iets verlegens/loops in (60b&c) refers to some aspect of the behavior of Peter/the dog, and iets zwangers in (60d) refers to Marieʼs way of walking.
a. | Jan heeft | iets dronkens | over zich. | |
Jan has | something drunk | about him |
b. | Er | zit | iets verlegens | in Peters gedrag. | |
there | is | something shy | in Peterʼs behavior |
c. | Er | zit | iets | loops | in het gedrag | van de hond. | |
there | is | something | in.season | in the behavior | of the dog |
d. | ? | Er | zit | iets zwangers | in Maries manier van lopen. |
there | is | something pregnant | in Marieʼs way of walking |
Note that the constructions with the verb zitten in (60b-d) alternate with the construction with the verb hebben'to have' in (61), in which the entity to which the partitive genitive construction attributes the relevant property appears as the subject of the clause. The examples in (60 ) and (61) clearly deserve more attention in the future; see Schoorlemmer (2005) for some discussion.
a. | Peters gedrag | heeft | iets verlegens. | |
Peterʼs behavior | has | something shy |
b. | Het gedrag van de hond | heeft | iets | loops. | |
the behavior of the dog | has | something | in.season |
c. | ? | Maries manier van lopen | heeft | iets zwangers. |
Marieʼs way of walking | has | something pregnant |
The exclusion of adjectives that modify +animate nouns only is probably related to the fact that the quantifiers iemand'someone' and niemand'no one' cannot be used as the nominal part of a partitive genitive construction. Moreover, the partitive genitive construction as a whole never refers to a +animate entity: iets slims'something smart' denotes a thing, e.g., a plan, not an animate being. The examples in (62) illustrate this again by showing that the predicatively used partitive genitive construction iets leuks can be predicated of a -animate noun phrase such as een feest'a party' but not over a +animate noun phrase like die man'that man'. This can be accounted for if we assume that the features of the nominal predicate and its subject must match, from which it follows that the partitive genitive construction has the feature -animate.
a. | Dat feest | wordt | iets leuks. | |
that party | becomes | something nice |
b. | * | Die man | is iets leuks. |
the man | is something nice |
Constructions like those in (63) can of course be found, but these assertions are offensive given that they represent the +human subject as an object. The primed examples show that replacement of the indefinite noun phrase by a proper noun or a referential pronoun renders the examples unacceptable.
a. | Een slaaf | is iets onmisbaars. | |
a slave | is something indispensable |
a'. | * | Jan/Hij | is iets onmisbaars. |
Jan/he | is something indispensable |
b. | Een vrouw | is iets ongrijpbaars. | |
a woman | is something impalpable |
b'. | * | Marie/Zij | is iets ongrijpbaars. |
Marie/she | is something impalpable |
Finally, it can be noted that adjectives that take a PP-complement can only be used as a partitive genitive if the PP can precede the adjective; cf. the discussion of (8) and (9) in Section 7.1. Since Section 2.1, sub I, has shown that adjectives like these generally select a +animatesubject, it does not come as a surprise that they hardly ever occur in the partitive genitive construction.
The examples in (64) give examples from the second subset of set-denoting adjective that cannot be used in the partitive genitive construction. These involve adjectives like jammer'a pity', mogelijk'possible' and zeker'certain', which normally take a proposition as their logical subject; Section 6.5 has shown that the proposition is normally expressed by a clausal subject preceded by the anticipatory non-referential pronoun het'it', or referred to by the anaphoric neuter demonstrative dit/dat'this/that'.
a. | [Dat Anke ziek wordt] | is mogelijk. | |
that Anke ill becomes | is possible | ||
'Itʼs possible that Anke will be ill.' |
a'. | *? | iets mogelijks |
something possible |
b. | [Dat Jan er morgen niet is] | is jammer. | |
that Jan there tomorrow not is | is a pity | ||
'Itʼs a pity that Jan wonʼt be there tomorrow.' |
b'. | * | iets jammers |
something pitiful |
In contrast to the adjective mogelijk in (64a), the adjective onmogelijk may take a noun phrase as its subject, and, as expected, it can also appear in the partitive genitive construction.
a. | Jans gedrag | is | (volstrekt) | onmogelijk/*mogelijk. | |
Janʼs behavior | is | completely | impossible/possible | ||
'Janʼs behavior work is completely unacceptable.' |
b. | iets (volstrekt) onmogelijks |
The third subclass consists of adjectives like bewolkt'cloudy', regenachtig'rainy' and benauwd'hard to breathe' that take so-called weather het as their subject in predicative structures. Some examples are given in (66).
a. | een | bewolkte | dag | |
a | cloudy | day |
a'. | Het | is bewolkt. | |
it | is cloudy |
a''. | * | iets bewolkts |
something cloudy |
b. | regenachtig | weer | |
rainy | weather |
b'. | Het | is regenachtig. | |
it | is rainy |
b''. | * | iets regenachtigs |
something rainy |
The fourth subclass consists of adjectives that take a nominal complement. Section 2.2 has shown that we should distinguish between adjectives that take a genitive and adjectives that take a dative complement; we will discuss these in separate subsections. A third subsection is devoted to adjectives with a nominal complement that cannot be used attributively.
Adjectives that take a genitive nominal complement, such as bewust'conscious', moe/zat/beu'tired', machtig'in command of', are always predicated of a +animate noun phrase. Consequently, these adjectives cannot occur as partitive genitives for the same reasons as those indicated in Subsection A above.
a. | Hij | is deze opera | zat. | |
he | is this opera | weary | ||
'Heʼs weary of this opera.' |
a'. | * | iets deze opera zats |
b. | Hij | is het Frans | machtig. | |
he | is the French | in.command.of | ||
'Heʼs able to speak French.' |
b'. | * | iets het Frans machtigs |
Adjectives that take a dative nominal complement, such as aangeboren'innate', bespaard'spared', duidelijk'clear', (on)bekend'(un)known', vreemd'foreign' and vertrouwd'familiar', may be predicated of a -animate noun phrase.
a. | Deze omgeving | is Peter erg vertrouwd. | |
this area | is Peter very familiar | ||
'This area is very familiar to Peter.' |
b. | De Universele Grammatica | is de mens | aangeboren. | |
the Universal Grammar | is the man | innate | ||
'Universal Grammar is innate to man.' |
c. | Deze oplossing | is Peter | onduidelijk. | |
this solution | is Peter | unclear | ||
'This solution is unclear to Peter.' |
Nevertheless, the primeless examples in (69) show that the partitive genitive use of these adjectives often leads to a degraded result. It should be noted, however, that the result improves somewhat if the noun phrase is replaced by a pronoun. The primed examples show that, if the dative noun phrase is optional, the partitive genitive constructions become fully acceptable if the noun phrase is dropped.
a. | iets | *Peter/?mij | vertrouwds | |
something | Peter/me | familiar |
a'. | iets vertrouwds |
b. | iets | *de mens/?ons | aangeborens | |
something | the man/us | innate |
b'. | iets aangeborens |
c. | iets | *Peter/?mij | onduidelijks | |
something | Peter/me | unclear |
c'. | iets onduidelijks |
Most gradable adjectives can also be combined with a dative nominal complement if the intensifierte'too' is added.
a. | Dat boek | is Peter te moeilijk. | |
that book | is Peter too difficult | ||
'That book is too difficult for Peter.' |
b. | Het water | is Marie | te koud. | |
the water | is Marie | too cold | ||
'The water is too cold for Marie.' |
Again, the partitive genitive use of the adjective is excluded if the noun phrase is present, although the same distinction between full noun phrases and pronouns arises as in the primeless examples in (69).
a. | iets | *Peter/?mij | te moeilijks | |
something | Peter/me | too difficult |
a'. | iets te moeilijks |
b. | iets | *Marie/?mij | te kouds | |
something | Marie/me | too cold |
b'. | iets te kouds |
Some adjectives that take a nominal complement can be used as a complementive only; cf. Section 6.2.3, sub V. As expected, the partitive genitive use of these adjectives is not possible.
a. | Hij | is zijn trui | kwijt. | |
he | is his sweater | lost | ||
'He has lost his sweater.' |
a'. | * | iets | kwijts |
something | lost |
b. | Hij | is het spoor | bijster. | |
he | is the track | lost | ||
'He lost his way.' |
b'. | * | iets | bijsters |
something | lost |
It is not clear, however, whether this must be attributed to the fact that the adjectives select a nominal argument or to the fact that they cannot be used attributively. The latter is suggested by the fact that adjectives like braak'fallow' and gelegen'convenient' in (73), which are like the adjectives in (72) in that they can be used predicatively only but unlike them in that they do not select a nominal complement, cannot be used in the partitive genitive construction either. Other examples are afhandig maken'deprive of' and gewaar worden'to become aware'. Note that the examples in this subsection are all more or less fixed expressions.
a. | Dit weiland | ligt braak. | |
this meadow | lies fallow |
a'. | * | iets | braaks |
something | fallow |
b. | dit boek | komt | gelegen | |
this book | comes | convenient |
b'. | * | iets | gelegens |
something | convenient |
The final subclass consists of the (absolute) superlatives. Example (74c'') shows that superlatives are excluded from the partitive genitive construction, whereas their corresponding positive and comparative forms are fully acceptable. The examples in (74d&e) show that periphrastic comparatives and superlatives behave just like the morphological comparatives in the doubly-primed examples in (74b&c).
a. | een leuk boek | |
a nice book |
a'. | Dit boek is leuk. | |
this book is nice |
a''. | iets leuks | |
something nice |
b. | een leuker boek | |
a nicer book |
b'. | Dit boek is leuker. | |
this book is nicer |
b''. | iets leukers | |
something nicer |
c. | het leukste boek | |
the nicest book |
c'. | Dit boek is het leukst. | |
this book the nicest |
c''. | * | iets (het)leuksts |
something nicest |
d. | een minder leuk boek | |
a less nice book |
d'. | Dit boek is minder leuk. | |
this book is less nice |
d''. | iets minder leuks | |
something less nice |
e. | het minst leuke boek | |
the least nice book |
e'. | Dit boek is het minst leuk. | |
this book is the least nice |
e''. | * | iets (het) minst leuks |
someth. the least nice |
We will see below, however, that we cannot immediately conclude from the unacceptability of (74c''&e'') that superlatives cannot occur as partitive genitives. First, observe that the predicatively used adjectives in (74c'&e') are preceded by the determiner(-like) element het. We have seen in Section 4.3.2, however, that there are superlative forms preceded by aller- that can be used as a predicate without het, the so-called pseudo-superlative. The English renderings in (75) show that the presence or absence of het corresponds to a semantic difference: alleraardigst in (75a) is preceded by het and the copular construction expresses that Jan has the property of being kind to the highest degree; alleraardigst in (75b), on the other hand, is not preceded by het, and the copular construction expresses that Jan has the property of being kind to a very high degree.
a. | Jan is het alleraardigst. | |
Jan is the aller-nicest | ||
'Jan is the nicest.' |
b. | Jan is alleraardigst. | |
Jan is aller-nicest | ||
'Jan is very nice.' |
Now consider the examples in (76), in which the pseudo-superlatives are predicated of -animatesubjectsand can appear as partitive genitives. The form of the partitive genitive is, however, rather special. The expected form A-sts is reduced to A-s: in (76a), for instance, the expected form allerleuksts surfaces as allerleuks, cf. Paardekooper (1986:691). Note, however, that some speakers have difficulty with both forms in the primed examples.
a. | Dit boek | is allerleukst. | |
this book | is very nice |
a'. | iets allerleuks/*?allerleuksts |
b. | Dit verhaal | is alleraardigst. | |
this story | is very nice |
b'. | iets alleraardigs/*?alleraardigsts |
Although the phonological reduction in the primed examples in (76) does not apply with partitive genitives like juists/onbewusts in iets juists/onbewusts'something right/unconscious', where the sequence /sts/ is the result of adding the -s ending to an adjective ending in /st/, it is familiar from other cases as well; cf. Section 4.1.2, sub I. If the phonological reduction should also apply to the absolute superlative forms in (74c&e), the form leuksts would be reduced to leuks. This implies that the partitive genitive leuks should be the corresponding partitive genitive of both the positive degree and the absolute superlative. Given that the partitive genitive construction iets leuks semantically corresponds to the positive degree only, we can now finally conclude with confidence that there are no partitive genitives that correspond to the absolute superlative.
The fact that it is impossible for the absolute superlatives to occur as partitive genitives may be related to the fact that these forms cannot be used as such in the copular construction, but must be preceded by the element het. Alternatively, we may assume that the fact that partitive genitive constructions are always indefinite is incompatible with the fact that absolute superlatives pick out a fixed entity or group of entities from the domain of discourse; they are in a sense inherently definite, which is also clear from the fact that they cannot be preceded by the indefinite article een if they are used attributively: *een leukste boek'a nicest book'.
Besides the systematic restrictions discussed in the previous subsections, additional phonological restrictions seem to play a role. The discussion has been restricted to so far partitive genitive adjectives that end in a consonant, but it should be noted that adjectives that end in a long vowel or a schwa often give rise to a marginal result in the partitive genitive construction. In (77), examples are given with adjectives that end in /a/, /o/, /i/, /e/, /y/ and a schwa. Observe, however, that some examples are more acceptable than others, and that judgments vary among speakers. It is not clear what determines the precise degree of acceptability; the fact that adjectives like these are generally loan words may play a role as well.
a. | * | iets prima-s | something excellent |
b. | ? | iets lila-s | something lilac |
c. | iets extra-s | something extra |
d. | * | iets albino-s | something albino |
e. | * | iets kaki-s | something khaki |
f. | ? | iets privé-s | something private |
g. | ? | iets continu-s | something continuous |
h. | ? | iets oranje-s | something orange |
For completeness’ sake, the examples in (78) show that the partitive genitive form of adjectives ending in /s/ has the same pronunciation as the corresponding positive form.
a. | vies | 'dirty' |
a'. | iets vies |
b. | paars | 'purple' |
b'. | iets paars |
c. | kleurloos | 'colorless' |
c'. | iets kleurloos |
Subsection I has shown that, with the exception of six well-defined classes, all set-denoting adjectives can be used as partitive genitive adjectives. The most important condition for entering the partitive genitive construction seems to be that the set-denoting adjective can be predicated of (or modify) -animate noun phrases. If we consider the relational adjectives, however, it turns out that many adjectives that may modify a -animate noun cannot be used in the partitive genitive construction. Some examples are given in (79).
a. | een Groninger koek | |
a Groninger cake |
a'. | * | Deze koek is Groninger. |
this cake is Groninger |
a''. | * | iets Groningers |
something Groninger |
b. | een houten bank | |
a wooden bench |
b'. | * | De bank is houten. |
this bench is wooden |
b''. | * | iets houtens |
something wooden |
Since the set-denoting and the relational adjectives differ with respect to acceptability if used as predicates, it might be the case that the question as to whether or not an adjective can occur as a partitive genitive is related to the question as to whether or not predicative use of this adjective is possible. If this is the case, we make the following prediction with respect to the relational adjectives. Since Section 1.3.3 has shown that a subset of the relational adjectives have the tendency to shift their meaning in the direction of the set-denoting adjectives, which is clear from their ability to occur in predicative position, we expect them to occur as partitive genitives as well; those relational adjectives that cannot occur in predicative position, on the other hand, are excluded from the partitive genitive construction. The discussion in the subsections below, which follows the classification of the relational adjectives in Section 1.3.3, will show that this expectation is indeed borne out.
Section 1.3.3, sub II, has distinguished the four classes of geographical adjectives shown in (80).
a. | derived from person nouns by means of -s: Amerikaans | 'American' |
b. | derived from person nouns by means of -isch: Russisch | 'Russian' |
c. | derived from Dutch geographical names by means of -er: Urker | 'from Urk' |
d. | other cases: buitengaats'offshore', ginds'yonder', plaatselijk'local', etc. |
With respect to the classes in (80a&b), we have seen that these adjectives tend to shift in the direction of the set-denoting adjectives and may therefore occur as predicates in certain contexts; this tendency can be enforced by the addition of the adverb typisch'typically'. Consequently, we correctly expect that these adjectives may also occur as partitive genitives; cf. (81).
a. | iets | (typisch) | Amerikaans/Nederlands | |
something | typically | American/Dutch |
b. | iets | (typisch) | Australisch/Russisch | |
something | typically | Australian/Russian |
Adjectives from the classes in (80c&d), on the other hand, are never used in predicative position and we therefore correctly predict their partitive genitive use to be impossible; cf. (82a).
a. | * | iets | (typisch) | Urkers/Groningers |
something | typically | Urker/Groninger |
b. | iets | *buitengaats/*ginds/?plaatselijks | |
something | offshore/over there/local |
Section 1.3.3, sub III, has divided the “movement/trend" adjectives into the three classes shown in (83).
a. | derived from person nouns by means of -s: freudiaans | 'Freudian' |
b. | derived from person nouns by means of -isch: kapitalistisch | 'capitalistic' |
c. | derived from person nouns by means of -er: dominicaner | 'Dominican' |
The classes in (83a&b) tend to shift their meaning in the direction of the set-denoting adjectives, which again can be enforced by means of adding the adverb typisch'typically'. The class in (83c), on the other hand, is never used in predicative position.
a. | Deze opvatting | is (typisch) | freudiaans. | |
this opinion | is typically | Freudian |
b. | ? | Deze opvatting | is (typisch) | kapitalistisch. |
this opinion | is typically | capitalistic |
c. | * | Dit klooster | is (typisch) | dominicaner. |
this monastery | is typically | Dominican |
Consequently, we expect that adjectives from the subclasses in (83a&b) can occur as partitive genitives, whereas this should be completely impossible with the adjectives from the class in (83c). The examples in (85) show that this expectation is indeed borne out.
a. | iets | (typisch) | freudiaans | |
something | typically | Freudian |
b. | iets | (typisch) | kapitalistisch | |
something | typically | capitalistic |
c. | * | iets | (typisch) | dominicaners |
something | typically | Dominican |
Section 1.3.3, sub IV, has divided the time adjectives into the three subclasses given in (86).
a. | derived from the nouns dag'day', week'week', maand'month', etc., by means of the suffix -(e)lijks: dagelijks'daily', wekelijks'weekly', maandelijks'monthly'derived from the nouns dag'day', week'week', maand'month', etc., by means of the suffix -(e)lijks: dagelijks'daily', wekelijks'weekly', maandelijks'monthly' |
b. | derived from the names of days, seasons (with the exception of lente'spring' and some months (especially maart'March') by means of the suffix -s. Such time adjectives can also be based on compounds and phrases, middeleeuws'medieval', zeventiende-eeuws'seventeenth-century'derived from the names of days, seasons (with the exception of lente'spring' and some months (especially maart'March') by means of the suffix -s. Such time adjectives can also be based on compounds and phrases, middeleeuws'medieval', zeventiende-eeuws'seventeenth-century' |
c. | other cases, which are mostly used as adverbs |
We start our discussion with the class in (86b). These adjectives generally cannot occur in predicative position, unless they refer to a certain, e.g., historical or cultural, period. Therefore, we expect these adjectives to occur in the partitive genitive construction on the latter reading only. This is indeed borne out.
a. | de | zaterdagse | bijlage | |
the | Saturday | supplement |
c. | een | middeleeuwse | opvatting | |
a | medieval | opinion |
a'. | * | De bijlage | is zaterdags. |
the supplement | is Saturday |
c'. | Deze opvatting | is (typisch) | middeleeuws. | |
this opinion | is typically | medieval |
a''. | *? | iets | zaterdags |
something | Saturday |
c''. | iets | (typisch) middeleeuws | |
something | typically medieval |
b. | maartse | buien | |
March | showers |
d. | een | zeventiende-eeuws | lied | |
a | seventeenth.century | song |
b'. | * | De buien | zijn | maarts. |
the showers | are | March |
d'. | Dit lied | is (typisch) | zeventiende-eeuws. | |
this song | is typically | seventeenth.century |
b''. | * | iets | maarts |
something | March |
d''. | iets | (typisch) | zeventiende-eeuws | |
something | typically | seventeenth.century |
Adjectives from the subclass in (86a) are at best marginally possible as predicates and they also yield marginal results in partitive genitive constructions such as (88c).
a. | zijn | maandelijkse | column | |
his | monthly | column |
b. | ?? | Zijn column | is maandelijks. |
his column | is monthly |
c. | ?? | Zijn column | is iets | maandelijks. |
his column | is something | monthly |
Adjectives from the subclass in (86c) are generally used as adverbs of time. Occasionally they also occur as attributive adjectives, but they are not readily used in predicative position. As expected, the judgment on the partitive genitive use of this type of adjective seems to correlate with the one on its predicative use. In (89) and (90), this is illustrated by means of the adjectives regelmatig'regular(ly)' and vroeger'previous(ly)'.
a. | Hij | klaagt | regelmatig. | |
he | complains | regularly |
b. | een | regelmatige | klacht | |
a | regular | complaint |
c. | *? | Deze klacht | is zeer regelmatig. |
this complaint | is very regular |
d. | *? | Zijn klacht | is iets regelmatigs. |
his complaint | is something regular |
a. | Dit | was vroeger | het stadhuis. | |
this | was previously | the city.hall | ||
'This used to be the city hall.' |
b. | het | vroegere | stadhuis | |
the | former | city.hall | ||
'the old city hall' |
c. | * | Het stadhuis | is vroeger. |
the city.hall | is former |
d. | * | Het stadhuis | is iets vroegers. |
the city.hall | is something former |
Substance adjectives can only be used in attributive position (cf. Section 1.3.3, sub V), and, as expected, the partitive genitive use of these adjectives is not possible.
a. | een | houten | kom | |
a | wooden | bowl |
b. | een | betonnen muur | |
a | concrete wall |
c. | een | zijden | draad | |
a | silk | thread |
a'. | * | Deze kom | is houten. |
this bowl | is wooden |
b'. | * | Deze muur | is betonnen. |
this wall | is concrete |
c'. | * | De draad | is zijden. |
the thread | is silk |
a''. | * | iets houtens/houts |
something wooden |
b''. | * | iets betonnens |
something concrete |
c''. | * | iets zijdens |
something silk |
For completeness’ sake, note that replacement of the substance adjectives in (91) by a van-PP makes the predicative constructions fully acceptable; this is illustrated for houten in (92a). This van-PP can also be added to the quantificational pronoun iets, as in (92b), but the structure of this construction is probably similar to the structure of the regular noun phrase in (92c).
a. | Deze kom | is van hout. | |
this bowl | is of wood |
b. | iets | van hout | |
something | of wood |
c. | een kom | van hout | |
a bowl | of wood |
Section 1.3.3, sub VI, has discussed some less systematic cases of denominal, relational adjectives. Again, these adjectives occasionally shift their meaning in the direction of a set-denoting adjective. If this is possible, partitive genitive use is possible as well: the partitive genitive construction in (93c) can refer to an administrative measure since the adjective administratief can be predicated of the noun maatregel in (93b).
a. | een | administratieve | maatregel | |
an | administrative | measure |
b. | Deze maatregel | is puur administratief. | |
this measure | is purely administrative |
c. | ? | Deze maatregel | is iets | puur | administratiefs. |
this measure | is something | purely | administrative |
The partitive genitive construction in (94c), on the other hand, cannot refer to a linguistic lexicon as the adjective taalkundig in (94b) cannot be predicated of the noun lexicon.
a. | een | taalkundig | lexicon | |
a | linguistic | lexicon |
b. | * | Dit lexicon | is taalkundig. |
this lexicon | is linguistic |
c. | *? | Dit lexicon | is iets | taalkundigs. |
this lexicon | is something | linguistic |
Evaluative adjectives such as drommels, duivels and bliksems cannot be used predicatively and the partitive genitive use of these adjectives is excluded in all cases; cf. the (a)-examples in (95). Positive evaluative adjectives such as hemels'heavenly' seem to be more adaptable to predicative use, and consequently also to partitive genitive use; cf. the (b)-examples in (95).
a. | die | drommelse | bout | |
that | devilish | bolt |
b. | een | hemels | plekje | |
a | heavenly | place |
a'. | * | Die bout | is/lijkt | drommels. |
that bolt | is/seems | devilish |
b'. | ? | Deze plek | is hemels. |
this place | is heavenly |
a''. | * | iets | drommels |
something | devilish |
b''. | ? | iets | hemels |
something | heavenly |
The residue consists of various classes of adjectives such as modal, amplifying, quantifying and restrictive adjectives. Modal adjectives such as potentieel'potential' and eventueel'possible' are never used as predicates, and their partitive genitive use is excluded as well.
a. | een | potentieel | tegenvoorbeeld | |
a | potential | counterexample |
b. | Peters | eventuele | vertrek | |
Peterʼs | possible | departure |
a'. | * | Dit tegenvoorbeeld | is potentieel. |
this counterexample | is potential |
b'. | * | Peters vertrek | is eventueel. |
Peterʼs departure | is possible |
a''. | * | iets | potentieels |
something | potential |
b''. | * | iets | eventueels |
something | possible |
The amplifying, quantifying and restrictive adjectives can often also be used as common set-denoting adjectives and therefore can also be found in partitive genitive constructions, but crucially not under the non-set denoting interpretation. Consider the examples in (97). The examples in (97a&b) illustrate the two relevant uses of the adjective duidelijk, which means something like “comprehensible" if used as a set-denoting adjective but something like “obvious" if used as an amplifying adjective. In the partitive genitive only the set-denoting interpretation survives; example (97d) illustrates this by showing that the amplifying meaning cannot even be triggered by using the partitive genitive construction as a nominal predicate that is predicated of a noun phrase that corresponds to the noun in the attributive construction in (97b).
a. | De tekst is duidelijk. | set-denoting | |
the text is comprehensible |
b. | de | duidelijke | fout | amplifying | |
the | obvious | mistake |
c. | iets | duidelijks | set-denoting | |
something | clear |
d. | * | Deze fout | is iets duidelijks. | amplifying |
this mistake | is something obvious |
The examples in (98) show the same for volledig, which has “complete" as its set-denoting meaning and “total" as its quantifying meaning, and enige, which has “unique/exceptional"as its set-denoting meaning and “only" as its restrictive meaning.
a. | De puzzel was volledig | |
the puzzle was complete |
a'. | een | volledige | onderwerping | |
a | total | submission |
a''. | ? | iets | volledigs |
something | complete |
a'''. | * | De onderwerping was iets volledigs |
the submission was something total |
b. | dat boek | was enig (in zijn soort) | |
that book | was unique/exceptional |
b'. | de | enige | gelegenheid | |
the | only | occasion |
b''. | iets | enigs (in zijn soort) | |
something | unique/exceptional |
b'''. | * | Deze gelegenheid | is iets enigs |
this occasion | is something only |
- 1986Beknopte ABN-syntaksisEindhovenP.C. Paardekooper
- 2005The status of the adjective in Dutch partitive constructions