- Dutch1
- Frisian
- Saterfrisian
- Afrikaans
-
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological processes
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Word stress
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Monomorphemic words
- Diachronic aspects
- Generalizations on stress placement
- Default penultimate stress
- Lexical stress
- The closed penult restriction
- Final closed syllables
- The diphthong restriction
- Superheavy syllables (SHS)
- The three-syllable window
- Segmental restrictions
- Phonetic correlates
- Stress shifts in loanwords
- Quantity-sensitivity
- Secondary stress
- Vowel reduction in unstressed syllables
- Stress in complex words
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Accent & intonation
- Clitics
- Spelling
- Morphology
- Word formation
- Compounding
- Nominal compounds
- Verbal compounds
- Adjectival compounds
- Affixoids
- Coordinative compounds
- Synthetic compounds
- Reduplicative compounds
- Phrase-based compounds
- Elative compounds
- Exocentric compounds
- Linking elements
- Separable complex verbs (SCVs)
- Gapping of complex words
- Particle verbs
- Copulative compounds
- Derivation
- Numerals
- Derivation: inputs and input restrictions
- The meaning of affixes
- Non-native morphology
- Cohering and non-cohering affixes
- Prefixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixation: person nouns
- Conversion
- Pseudo-participles
- Bound forms
- Nouns
- Nominal prefixes
- Nominal suffixes
- -aal and -eel
- -aar
- -aard
- -aat
- -air
- -aris
- -ast
- Diminutives
- -dom
- -een
- -ees
- -el (nominal)
- -elaar
- -enis
- -er (nominal)
- -erd
- -erik
- -es
- -eur
- -euse
- ge...te
- -heid
- -iaan, -aan
- -ief
- -iek
- -ier
- -ier (French)
- -ière
- -iet
- -igheid
- -ij and allomorphs
- -ijn
- -in
- -ing
- -isme
- -ist
- -iteit
- -ling
- -oir
- -oot
- -rice
- -schap
- -schap (de)
- -schap (het)
- -sel
- -st
- -ster
- -t
- -tal
- -te
- -voud
- Verbs
- Adjectives
- Adverbs
- Univerbation
- Neo-classical word formation
- Construction-dependent morphology
- Morphological productivity
- Compounding
- Inflection
- Inflection and derivation
- Allomorphy
- The interface between phonology and morphology
- Word formation
- Syntax
- Preface and acknowledgements
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of verb phrases I:Argument structure
- 3 Projection of verb phrases II:Verb frame alternations
- Introduction
- 3.1. Main types
- 3.2. Alternations involving the external argument
- 3.3. Alternations of noun phrases and PPs
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.3.1.1. Dative alternation with aan-phrases (recipients)
- 3.3.1.2. Dative alternation with naar-phrases (goals)
- 3.3.1.3. Dative alternation with van-phrases (sources)
- 3.3.1.4. Dative alternation with bij-phrases (possessors)
- 3.3.1.5. Dative alternation with voor-phrases (benefactives)
- 3.3.1.6. Conclusion
- 3.3.1.7. Bibliographical notes
- 3.3.2. Accusative/PP alternations
- 3.3.3. Nominative/PP alternations
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.4. Some apparent cases of verb frame alternation
- 3.5. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of verb phrases IIIa:Selection of clauses/verb phrases
- 5 Projection of verb phrases IIIb:Argument and complementive clauses
- Introduction
- 5.1. Finite argument clauses
- 5.2. Infinitival argument clauses
- 5.3. Complementive clauses
- 6 Projection of verb phrases IIIc:Complements of non-main verbs
- 7 Projection of verb phrases IIId:Verb clusters
- 8 Projection of verb phrases IV: Adverbial modification
- 9 Word order in the clause I:General introduction
- 10 Word order in the clause II:Position of the finite verb (verb-first/second)
- 11 Word order in the clause III:Clause-initial position (wh-movement)
- Introduction
- 11.1. The formation of V1- and V2-clauses
- 11.2. Clause-initial position remains (phonetically) empty
- 11.3. Clause-initial position is filled
- 12 Word order in the clause IV:Postverbal field (extraposition)
- 13 Word order in the clause V: Middle field (scrambling)
- 14 Main-clause external elements
- Nouns and Noun Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of noun phrases I: complementation
- Introduction
- 2.1. General observations
- 2.2. Prepositional and nominal complements
- 2.3. Clausal complements
- 2.4. Bibliographical notes
- 3 Projection of noun phrases II: modification
- Introduction
- 3.1. Restrictive and non-restrictive modifiers
- 3.2. Premodification
- 3.3. Postmodification
- 3.3.1. Adpositional phrases
- 3.3.2. Relative clauses
- 3.3.3. Infinitival clauses
- 3.3.4. A special case: clauses referring to a proposition
- 3.3.5. Adjectival phrases
- 3.3.6. Adverbial postmodification
- 3.4. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of noun phrases III: binominal constructions
- Introduction
- 4.1. Binominal constructions without a preposition
- 4.2. Binominal constructions with a preposition
- 4.3. Bibliographical notes
- 5 Determiners: articles and pronouns
- Introduction
- 5.1. Articles
- 5.2. Pronouns
- 5.3. Bibliographical notes
- 6 Numerals and quantifiers
- 7 Pre-determiners
- Introduction
- 7.1. The universal quantifier al 'all' and its alternants
- 7.2. The pre-determiner heel 'all/whole'
- 7.3. A note on focus particles
- 7.4. Bibliographical notes
- 8 Syntactic uses of noun phrases
- Adjectives and Adjective Phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- 2 Projection of adjective phrases I: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adjective phrases II: Modification
- 4 Projection of adjective phrases III: Comparison
- 5 Attributive use of the adjective phrase
- 6 Predicative use of the adjective phrase
- 7 The partitive genitive construction
- 8 Adverbial use of the adjective phrase
- 9 Participles and infinitives: their adjectival use
- 10 Special constructions
- Adpositions and adpositional phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Introduction
- 1.1. Characterization of the category adposition
- 1.2. A formal classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3. A semantic classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3.1. Spatial adpositions
- 1.3.2. Temporal adpositions
- 1.3.3. Non-spatial/temporal prepositions
- 1.4. Borderline cases
- 1.5. Bibliographical notes
- 2 Projection of adpositional phrases: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adpositional phrases: Modification
- 4 Syntactic uses of the adpositional phrase
- 5 R-pronominalization and R-words
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Phonology
-
- General
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological Processes
- Assimilation
- Vowel nasalization
- Syllabic sonorants
- Final devoicing
- Fake geminates
- Vowel hiatus resolution
- Vowel reduction introduction
- Schwa deletion
- Schwa insertion
- /r/-deletion
- d-insertion
- {s/z}-insertion
- t-deletion
- Intrusive stop formation
- Breaking
- Vowel shortening
- h-deletion
- Replacement of the glide w
- Word stress
- Clitics
- Allomorphy
- Orthography of Frisian
- Morphology
- Inflection
- Word formation
- Derivation
- Prefixation
- Infixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixes
- Verbal suffixes
- Adjectival suffixes
- Adverbial suffixes
- Numeral suffixes
- Interjectional suffixes
- Onomastic suffixes
- Conversion
- Compositions
- Derivation
- Syntax
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Unergative and unaccusative subjects
- Evidentiality
- To-infinitival clauses
- Predication and noun incorporation
- Ellipsis
- Imperativus-pro-Infinitivo
- Expression of irrealis
- Embedded Verb Second
- Agreement
- Negation
- Nouns & Noun Phrases
- Classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Partitive noun constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Nominalised quantifiers
- Kind partitives
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Bare nominal attributions
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers and (pre)determiners
- Interrogative pronouns
- R-pronouns
- Syntactic uses
- Adjective Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification and degree quantification
- Comparison by degree
- Comparative
- Superlative
- Equative
- Attribution
- Agreement
- Attributive adjectives vs. prenominal elements
- Complex adjectives
- Noun ellipsis
- Co-occurring adjectives
- Predication
- Partitive adjective constructions
- Adverbial use
- Participles and infinitives
- Adposition Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Intransitive adpositions
- Predication
- Preposition stranding
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
-
- General
- Morphology
- Morphology
- 1 Word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 1.1.1 Compounds and their heads
- 1.1.2 Special types of compounds
- 1.1.2.1 Affixoids
- 1.1.2.2 Coordinative compounds
- 1.1.2.3 Synthetic compounds and complex pseudo-participles
- 1.1.2.4 Reduplicative compounds
- 1.1.2.5 Phrase-based compounds
- 1.1.2.6 Elative compounds
- 1.1.2.7 Exocentric compounds
- 1.1.2.8 Linking elements
- 1.1.2.9 Separable Complex Verbs and Particle Verbs
- 1.1.2.10 Noun Incorporation Verbs
- 1.1.2.11 Gapping
- 1.2 Derivation
- 1.3 Minor patterns of word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 2 Inflection
- 1 Word formation
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
- 0 Introduction to the AP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of APs
- 2 Complementation of APs
- 3 Modification and degree quantification of APs
- 4 Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative
- 5 Attribution of APs
- 6 Predication of APs
- 7 The partitive adjective construction
- 8 Adverbial use of APs
- 9 Participles and infinitives as APs
- Nouns and Noun Phrases (NPs)
- 0 Introduction to the NP
- 1 Characteristics and Classification of NPs
- 2 Complementation of NPs
- 3 Modification of NPs
- 3.1 Modification of NP by Determiners and APs
- 3.2 Modification of NP by PP
- 3.3 Modification of NP by adverbial clauses
- 3.4 Modification of NP by possessors
- 3.5 Modification of NP by relative clauses
- 3.6 Modification of NP in a cleft construction
- 3.7 Free relative clauses and selected interrogative clauses
- 4 Partitive noun constructions and constructions related to them
- 4.1 The referential partitive construction
- 4.2 The partitive construction of abstract quantity
- 4.3 The numerical partitive construction
- 4.4 The partitive interrogative construction
- 4.5 Adjectival, nominal and nominalised partitive quantifiers
- 4.6 Kind partitives
- 4.7 Partitive predication with a preposition
- 4.8 Bare nominal attribution
- 5 Articles and names
- 6 Pronouns
- 7 Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- 8 Interrogative pronouns
- 9 R-pronouns and the indefinite expletive
- 10 Syntactic functions of Noun Phrases
- Adpositions and Adpositional Phrases (PPs)
- 0 Introduction to the PP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of PPs
- 2 Complementation of PPs
- 3 Modification of PPs
- 4 Bare (intransitive) adpositions
- 5 Predication of PPs
- 6 Form and distribution of adpositions with respect to staticity and construction type
- 7 Adpositional complements and adverbials
- Verbs and Verb Phrases (VPs)
- 0 Introduction to the VP in Saterland Frisian
- 1 Characteristics and classification of verbs
- 2 Unergative and unaccusative subjects and the auxiliary of the perfect
- 3 Evidentiality in relation to perception and epistemicity
- 4 Types of to-infinitival constituents
- 5 Predication
- 5.1 The auxiliary of being and its selection restrictions
- 5.2 The auxiliary of going and its selection restrictions
- 5.3 The auxiliary of continuation and its selection restrictions
- 5.4 The auxiliary of coming and its selection restrictions
- 5.5 Modal auxiliaries and their selection restrictions
- 5.6 Auxiliaries of body posture and aspect and their selection restrictions
- 5.7 Transitive verbs of predication
- 5.8 The auxiliary of doing used as a semantically empty finite auxiliary
- 5.9 Supplementive predication
- 6 The verbal paradigm, irregularity and suppletion
- 7 Verb Second and the word order in main and embedded clauses
- 8 Various aspects of clause structure
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
-
- General
- Phonology
- Afrikaans phonology
- Segment inventory
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- The diphthongised long vowels /e/, /ø/ and /o/
- The unrounded mid-front vowel /ɛ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /ɑ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /a/
- The rounded mid-high back vowel /ɔ/
- The rounded high back vowel /u/
- The rounded and unrounded high front vowels /i/ and /y/
- The unrounded and rounded central vowels /ə/ and /œ/
- The diphthongs /əi/, /œy/ and /œu/
- Overview of Afrikaans consonants
- The bilabial plosives /p/ and /b/
- The alveolar plosives /t/ and /d/
- The velar plosives /k/ and /g/
- The bilabial nasal /m/
- The alveolar nasal /n/
- The velar nasal /ŋ/
- The trill /r/
- The lateral liquid /l/
- The alveolar fricative /s/
- The velar fricative /x/
- The labiodental fricatives /f/ and /v/
- The approximants /ɦ/, /j/ and /ʋ/
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- Word stress
- The phonetic properties of stress
- Primary stress on monomorphemic words in Afrikaans
- Background to primary stress in monomorphemes in Afrikaans
- Overview of the Main Stress Rule of Afrikaans
- The short vowels of Afrikaans
- Long vowels in monomorphemes
- Primary stress on diphthongs in monomorphemes
- Exceptions
- Stress shifts in place names
- Stress shift towards word-final position
- Stress pattern of reduplications
- Phonological processes
- Vowel related processes
- Consonant related processes
- Homorganic glide insertion
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Phonotactics
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Afrikaans syntax
- Nouns and noun phrases
- Characteristics of the NP
- Classification of nouns
- Complementation of NPs
- Modification of NPs
- Binominal and partitive constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Partitive constructions with nominalised quantifiers
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Binominal name constructions
- Binominal genitive constructions
- Bare nominal attribution
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- Syntactic uses of the noun phrase
- Adjectives and adjective phrases
- Characteristics and classification of the AP
- Complementation of APs
- Modification and Degree Quantification of APs
- Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative degree
- Attribution of APs
- Predication of APs
- The partitive adjective construction
- Adverbial use of APs
- Participles and infinitives as adjectives
- Verbs and verb phrases
- Characterisation and classification
- Argument structure
- Verb frame alternations
- Complements of non-main verbs
- Verb clusters
- Complement clauses
- Adverbial modification
- Word order in the clause: Introduction
- Word order in the clause: position of the finite Verb
- Word order in the clause: Clause-initial position
- Word order in the clause: Extraposition and right-dislocation in the postverbal field
- Word order in the middle field
- Emphatic constructions
- Adpositions and adposition phrases
Infinitival argument clauses preceded by om have more or less the same distribution as finite argument clauses (cf. Section 5.1); they can occur as direct objects, subjects and prepositional objects, but indirect object clauses are rare given that these usually refer to animate objects and/or institutions. Furthermore, om + te-infinitivals normally follow the verb(s) in clause-final position, although there are limited possibilities for them to be topicalized or left-dislocated.
Object clauses normally follow the verb(s) in clause-final position and can be optionally preceded by the anticipatory object pronoun het'it'; placing the object clause in the middle field of the clause generally leads to a highly degraded result. This is illustrated in the (a)-examples in (347) for finite and by the (b)-examples for infinitival clauses.
a. | dat | Jan | (het) | besloot | [dat | hij | het boek | zou | kopen]. | |
that | Jan | it | decided | that | he | the book | would | buy | ||
'that Jan decided (it) that he would buy the book.' |
a'. | *? | dat Jan [dat hij het boek zou kopen] besloot. |
b. | dat | Jan | (het) | besloot | [(om) PRO | het boek | te kopen]. | |
that | Jan | it | decided | comp | the book | to buy | ||
'that Jan decided to buy the book.' |
b'. | * | dat Jan [(om) PRO het boek te kopen] besloot. |
For completeness' sake, the examples in (348) show that the object clauses of the verb besluiten'to decide' can also appear as the complement of the corresponding nominalization besluit'decision'.
a. | het besluit | [dat | hij | het boek | zou | kopen] | |
the decision | that | he | the book | would | buy | ||
'the decision that he would buy the book' |
b. | het besluit | [(om) PRO | het boek | te kopen] | |
the decision | comp | the book | to buy | ||
'the decision to buy the book' |
In (347), the object clause is an internal argument of the verb besluiten'to decide'. Direct object clauses can, however, also function as logical subjects (external arguments) of adjectival complementives. This is illustrated by means of the vinden-construction in (349); in these examples the anticipatory pronounhet is obligatory and the object clause normally follows the verb(s) in clause-final position—placement of the object clause in the middle field leads to a severely degraded result.
a. | dat | Jan | *(het) | vervelend | vindt | [dat | hij | niet | kan | komen]. | |
that | Jan | it | annoying | considers | that | he | not | is.able | come | ||
'that Jan considers it annoying that he isnʼt able to come.' |
a'. | *? | dat Jan [dat hij niet kan to komen] vervelend vindt. |
b. | dat | Jan | *(het) | vervelend | vindt | [(om) PRO | niet | te kunnen | komen]. | |
that | Jan | it | annoying | considers | comp | not | to be.able | come | ||
'that Jan considers it annoying not to be able to come.' |
b'. | * | dat Jan [(om) PRO niet te kunnen komen] vervelend vindt. |
Subject clauses are possible if they originate as internal arguments of the matrix verb, as is clear from the fact illustrated in (350) that the primeless examples of the transitive constructions in (347) can be passivized. The passive construction can be impersonal, that is, introduced by the expletiveer'there', or personal, that is, contain the anticipatory subject pronoun het'it'. The primed examples in (350) show that subject clauses must follow the verb(s) in clause-final position; they cannot be placed in the regular subject position right-adjacent to the complementizer.
a. | dat | er/het | besloten werd | [dat | hij | het boek | zou | kopen]. | |
that | there/it | decided was | that | he | the book | would | buy | ||
'that it was decided that he would buy the book.' |
a'. | *? | dat [dat hij het boek zou kopen] besloten werd. |
b. | dat | er/het | besloten | werd | [(om) PRO | het boek | te kopen]. | |
that | there/it | decided | was | comp | the book | to buy | ||
'that it was decided to buy the book.' |
b'. | * | dat [(om) PRO het boek te kopen] besloten werd. |
Subject clauses also occur in dyadic unaccusative constructions, in which they likewise originate as internal arguments of the verb. This is illustrated by means of the nom-dat object experiencer psych-verb bevallen'to please' in (351); the primed examples show again that subject clauses cannot occur in the regular subject position. In these examples the anticipatory pronoun is obligatory and the expletive er cannot be used.
a. | dat | het | me niet | bevalt | [dat | hij | steeds | dezelfde vraag | stelt]. | |
that | it | me not | pleases | that | he | constantly | the.same question | poses | ||
'that it displeases me that heʼs asking the same question all the time.' |
a'. | *? | dat [dat hij steeds dezelfde vraag stelt] me niet bevalt. |
b. | dat | het | me niet bevalt | [(om) PRO | steeds | dezelfde vraag | te stellen]. | |
that | it | me not pleases | comp | constantly | the.same question | to pose | ||
'that it displeases me to ask the same question all the time.' |
b'. | *? | dat [om PRO steeds dezelfde vraag te stellen] me niet bevalt. |
The examples in (352) show that the same thing holds for the nom-acc object experiencer psych-verb vervelen'to annoy', provided that the subject functions as a cause (and not as a causer) argument; see Section 2.5.1.3 for an extensive discussion of these psych-verbs.
a. | dat | het | me verveelt | [dat | hij | steeds | dezelfde | vraag | stelt]cause. | |
that | it | me annoys | that | he | constantly | the.same | question | poses | ||
'that it annoys me that heʼs asking the same question all the time.' |
a'. | *? | dat [dat hij steeds dezelfde vraag stelt] me verveelt. |
b. | dat | het | me verveelt | [(om) PRO | steeds | dezelfde vraag | te stellen]. | |
that | it | me annoys | comp | constantly | the.same question | to pose | ||
'that it annoys me to ask the same question all the time.' |
b'. | * | dat [om PRO steeds dezelfde vraag te stellen] me verveelt. |
In the examples above, the subject clause is an argument of the matrix verb, subject clauses can, however, also function as logical subjects (external arguments) of adjectival complementives, which is clear from the fact illustrated in (353) that the vinden-constructions in (349) can be passivized. The anticipatory pronoun het is normally obligatory and surfaces as the subject of the construction; placement of the subject clause in the regular subject position is impossible. Again, the expletive er cannot be used.
a. | dat | het | vervelend | gevonden | wordt | [dat | hij | niet | kan | komen]. | |
that | it | annoying | considered | is | that | he | not | is.able.to | come | ||
'that it is considered annoying that he isnʼt able to come.' |
a'. | *? | dat [dat hij niet kan komen] vervelend gevonden wordt. |
b. | dat | het | vervelend | gevonden wordt | [(om) PRO | niet | te kunnen | komen]. | |
that | it | annoying | considered is | comp | not | to be.able.to | come | ||
'that it is considered annoying not to be able to come.' |
b'. | * | dat [(om) PRO niet te kunnen komen] vervelend gevonden wordt. |
The same thing is illustrated by the copular constructions in (349); again, the anticipatory pronoun het is normally obligatory and it is impossible to place the subject clause in the regular subject position.
a. | dat | het | vervelend | is | [dat | hij | niet | kan | komen]. | |
that | it | annoying | is | that | he | not | is.able.to | come | ||
'that it is annoying that he isnʼt able to come.' |
a'. | *? | dat [dat hij niet kan komen] vervelend is. |
b. | dat | het | vervelend | is | [(om) PRO | niet | te kunnen | komen]. | |
that | it | annoying | is | comp | not | to be.able.to | come | ||
'that it is annoying not to be able to come.' |
b'. | * | dat [(om) PRO niet te kunnen komen] vervelend is. |
The examples in (355) show that finite and infinitival clauses can also be used as PO-clauses, in which case they can be introduced by an anticipatory pronominal PPer + P. This pronominal PP can be omitted with certain verbs but not with all; see Section 2.3.1, sub VI, for detailed discussion. The primed examples show that complement clauses cannot appear in the middle field of the clause, regardless of whether or not ernaar is present.
a. | dat | Jan | (ernaar) | verlangt | [dat | hij | weer | thuis | is]. | |
that | Jan | for.it | craves | that | he | again | home | is | ||
'that Jan wishes that heʼs home again.' |
a'. | *? | dat Jan (naar) [dat hij weer thuis is] verlangt. |
b. | dat | Jan | (ernaar) | verlangt | [(om) PRO | weer | thuis | te zijn]. | |
that | Jan | for.it | craves | comp | again | home | to be | ||
'that Jan longs to be home again.' |
b'. | * | dat Jan (naar) [om PRO weer thuis te zijn] verlangt. |
For completeness' sake, the examples in (356) show that finite and infinitival clauses can also be used as prepositional complements of adjectives. The pronominal PP can be omitted with certain adjectives but not with all, and the complement clause cannot appear in the middle field of the clause.
a. | dat | Jan (er) | bang | (voor) | is | [dat | hij | te laat | komt]. | |
that | Jan there | afraid | of | is | that | he | too late | comes | ||
'that Jan is afraid (of it) that heʼll be late.' |
a'. | * | dat Jan bang (voor) [dat hij te laat komt] is. |
b. | Jan is (er) | bang | (voor) | [(om) PRO | te laat | te komen]. | |
Jan is there | afraid | of | comp | too late | to come | ||
'Jan is afraid (of it) to be late.' |
b'. | * | dat Jan bang (voor) [(om) PRO te laat te komen] is. |
Interestingly, anticipatory pronominal PPs do not occur in noun phrases. The nominalizations of the primeless examples in (357) can only be combined with the pronominal PP ernaar if the clause is not realized. For completeness' sake, note that for some reason, nominalization gives rise to a somewhat marked result when the complement is a finite clause.
a. | het verlangen | ?(*ernaar) | [dat | hij | weer | thuis | is] | |
the craving | for.it | that | he | again | at.home | is |
b. | het verlangen | (*ernaar) | [(om) PRO | weer | thuis | te zijn] | |
the craving | for.it | comp | again | at.home | to be |
c. | het verlangen | (ernaar) | |
the craving | for.it |
The previous subsections have shown that infinitival argument clauses preceded by om behave like their finite counterparts in that they normally follow the verb(s) in clause-final position, that is, that they cannot occur in the middle field of the clause. It is, however, possible to topicalize or left-dislocate the infinitival clause, although the options seem a bit more limited than in the case of finite argument clauses.
Example (358a) shows that topicalizing a finite object clause is quite normal (provided that the anticipatory pronoun het is omitted), but that this leads to a marked result in the case of an infinitival clause; for some speakers examples such as (358b) improve when emphatic accent is assigned to some element in the infinitival clause, in this case boek'book'.
a. | [Dat | hij | het boek | zou | kopen] | besloot | hij | snel. | |
that | he | the book | would | buy | decided | he | quickly | ||
'That he would buy the book he decided quickly.' |
b. | *? | [(Om) PRO | het boek | te kopen] | besloot | hij | snel. |
comp | the book | to buy | decided | he | quickly |
The contrast disappears, however, in left-dislocation constructions, especially if there is some contrastively focused element in the left-dislocated clause. We illustrate this in (359) by means of contrastive accent on the direct object het boek'the book', but it might equally well have been on the main verb kopen'to buy'.
a. | [Dat | hij | het boek | zou | kopen], | dat | besloot | hij | snel. | |
that | he | the book | would | buy | that | decided | he | quickly | ||
'That he would buy the book, that he decided quickly.' |
b. | [(om) PRO | het boek | te kopen], | dat | besloot | hij | snel. | |
comp | the book | to buy | that | decided | he | quickly |
A problem for passing judgments on the examples in (358) is that the resumptive pronoun dat in (359) is optional, as a result of which the distinction between topicalization and left dislocation rests entirely on intonation and meaning. First, topicalized phrases are typically part of a larger prosodic unit, including the finite verb in second position, while left-dislocated phrases typically constitute a prosodic unit on their own. Second, topicalized phrases typically refer to known information, whereas left-dislocated phrases typically refer to new or contrastively focused information.
Subsection II has shown that subject clauses cannot occur in the regular subject position. This was illustrated by showing that such clauses cannot follow the complementizer in embedded clauses, and in (360) we show that they cannot follow the finite verb in second position either.
a. | Vaak | verveelt | het | me | [dat | hij | steeds | dezelfde vraag | stelt]. | |
often | annoys | it | me | that | he | constantly | the.same question | poses | ||
'It often annoys me that he always asks the same question.' |
a'. | *? | Vaak verveelt [dat hij steeds dezelfde vraag stelt] me. |
b. | Vaak verveelt | het | me | [(om) PRO | steeds | dezelfde vraag | te stellen]. | |
often annoys | it | me | comp | constantly | the.same question | to pose | ||
'It often annoys me to always ask the same question.' |
b'. | * | Vaak verveelt [(om) PRO steeds dezelfde vraag te stellen] me. |
In the literature we find different evaluations of examples in which infinitival subject clauses preceded by om occur in sentence-initial, that is, in topicalized position. Paardekooper (1986: 358) provides examples of the type in (361b) without any comment and it seems, indeed, that these are just as acceptable as examples such as (361a) with a finite subject clause.
a. | [Dat | hij | steeds | dezelfde | vraag | stelt]cause | verveelt | me. | |
that | he | constantly | the.same | question | poses | annoys | me | ||
'That he always asks the same question annoys me.' |
b. | [(Om) PRO | steeds | dezelfde vraag | te moeten stellen] | verveelt | me. | |
comp | constantly | the.same question | to have.to pose | annoys | me | ||
'Always having to ask the same question annoys me.' |
Dik (1985:35), on the other hand, claims that om + te-infinitivals of the type in (362b) are quite marked, especially if the linker element om is present. It is, however, not so clear whether this observation is valid for all speakers since some of our informants do accept examples like these.
a. | [Dat | hij | hier | zwemt] | is gevaarlijk. | |
that | he | here | swims | is dangerous | ||
'That he swims here is dangerous.' |
b. | % | [(Om) | hier | te zwemmen] | is gevaarlijk. |
comp | here | to swim | is dangerous |
In order to account for the contrast between the (b)-examples in (361) and (362), we might of course hypothesize that the prohibition against topicalization of infinitival subject clauses is restricted to cases in which the nominative subject is not an argument of the verb but the logical subject of a complementive adjective. However, this seems to go against Paardekooper's (1985:117) judgment of example (363b), which does seem to have a similar status as example (363a) with a finite subject clause.
a. | [Dat | ik | even | moest | wachten] | was | niet | zo vervelend. | |
that | I | a.while | had.to | wait | was | not | so annoying | ||
'That I had to wait a while wasnʼt so annoying.' |
b. | [Om | even | te moeten | wachten] | was niet | zo vervelend. | |
comp | a.while | to have.to | wait | was not | so annoying | ||
'To have to wait a while wasnʼt so annoying.' |
For the moment, we therefore have to conclude that Dik's categorical statement that infinitival clauses preceded by om cannot occupy the sentence-initial position is not supported by judgments of other speakers, and that infinitival subject clauses can in fact occupy this position (although they are cases which are judged as somewhat marked for unknown reasons). For completeness' sake, the examples in (364) show that left dislocation of infinitival subject clauses always gives rise to an impeccable result.
a. | [(Om) PRO | steeds | dezelfde vragen | te stellen], | dat | verveelt | me. | |
comp | constantly | the.same questions | to pose | that | annoys | me | ||
'To ask the same questions all the time, that annoys me.' |
b. | [(Om) | hier | te zwemmen], | dat | is gevaarlijk. | |
comp | here | to swim | that | is dangerous | ||
'To swim here, that is dangerous.' |
c. | [Om | even | te moeten | wachten], | dat | was niet | zo vervelend. | |
comp | a.while | to must | wait | that | was not | so annoying | ||
'To have to wait a while, that wasnʼt so annoying.' |
The primeless examples show that topicalization of PO-clauses is impossible, regardless of whether they are finite or infinitival. Left dislocation, on the other hand, gives rise to an impeccable result, as is shown by the primed examples.
a. | * | [Dat | hij | weer | thuis | is] | verlangt | Jan | (ernaar). |
that | he | again | at.home | is | craves | Jan | for.it |
a'. | [Dat | hij | weer | thuis | is], | daar | verlangt | Jan naar. | |
that | he | again | at.home | is | there | craves | Jan for | ||
'That heʼs home again, Jan longs for it.' |
b. | * | [(Om) PRO | weer | thuis | te zijn] | verlangt | Jan | (ernaar). |
comp | again | at.home | to be | craves | Jan | for.it |
b'. | [(Om) PRO | weer | thuis | te zijn], | daar | verlangt | Jan naar. | |
comp | again | at.home | to be | there | craves | Jan for | ||
'To be home again, Jan longs for it.' |
The previous subsections have shown that infinitival argument clauses exhibit syntactic behavior similar to finite argument clauses. First, they may function as subject, direct object and prepositional object. Second, they normally appear after the verb(s) in clause-final position and can be introduced by an anticipatory pronominal element in the middle field of the clause. The only difference seems to be related to topicalization; whereas topicalization of finite object clauses is easily possible, topicalization of object om + te infinitivals gives rise to degraded results. The same contrast has been claimed to hold for subject clauses but we have seen that there are many cases in which subject clauses can quite felicitously occur in sentence-initial position, and we therefore provisionally assumed that the reported contrast is not real. Finite and infinitival prepositional object clauses also behave in the same way in that they both resist topicalization. We also discussed left dislocation and showed that in this area there are no differences at all between finite clause and infinitival clauses preceded by om; left dislocation is always possible.
- 1985Infinitief-konstrukties met <i>om</i> in een functionele grammatikaGLOT825-46
- 1985Indeling van de <i>om</i>-zinnen en indelingen met behulp daarvanGLOT8127-128
- 1986Beknopte ABN-syntaksisEindhovenP.C. Paardekooper