- Dutch1
- Frisian
- Saterfrisian
- Afrikaans
-
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological processes
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Word stress
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Monomorphemic words
- Diachronic aspects
- Generalizations on stress placement
- Default penultimate stress
- Lexical stress
- The closed penult restriction
- Final closed syllables
- The diphthong restriction
- Superheavy syllables (SHS)
- The three-syllable window
- Segmental restrictions
- Phonetic correlates
- Stress shifts in loanwords
- Quantity-sensitivity
- Secondary stress
- Vowel reduction in unstressed syllables
- Stress in complex words
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Accent & intonation
- Clitics
- Spelling
- Morphology
- Word formation
- Compounding
- Nominal compounds
- Verbal compounds
- Adjectival compounds
- Affixoids
- Coordinative compounds
- Synthetic compounds
- Reduplicative compounds
- Phrase-based compounds
- Elative compounds
- Exocentric compounds
- Linking elements
- Separable complex verbs (SCVs)
- Gapping of complex words
- Particle verbs
- Copulative compounds
- Derivation
- Numerals
- Derivation: inputs and input restrictions
- The meaning of affixes
- Non-native morphology
- Cohering and non-cohering affixes
- Prefixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixation: person nouns
- Conversion
- Pseudo-participles
- Bound forms
- Nouns
- Nominal prefixes
- Nominal suffixes
- -aal and -eel
- -aar
- -aard
- -aat
- -air
- -aris
- -ast
- Diminutives
- -dom
- -een
- -ees
- -el (nominal)
- -elaar
- -enis
- -er (nominal)
- -erd
- -erik
- -es
- -eur
- -euse
- ge...te
- -heid
- -iaan, -aan
- -ief
- -iek
- -ier
- -ier (French)
- -ière
- -iet
- -igheid
- -ij and allomorphs
- -ijn
- -in
- -ing
- -isme
- -ist
- -iteit
- -ling
- -oir
- -oot
- -rice
- -schap
- -schap (de)
- -schap (het)
- -sel
- -st
- -ster
- -t
- -tal
- -te
- -voud
- Verbs
- Adjectives
- Adverbs
- Univerbation
- Neo-classical word formation
- Construction-dependent morphology
- Morphological productivity
- Compounding
- Inflection
- Inflection and derivation
- Allomorphy
- The interface between phonology and morphology
- Word formation
- Syntax
- Preface and acknowledgements
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of verb phrases I:Argument structure
- 3 Projection of verb phrases II:Verb frame alternations
- Introduction
- 3.1. Main types
- 3.2. Alternations involving the external argument
- 3.3. Alternations of noun phrases and PPs
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.3.1.1. Dative alternation with aan-phrases (recipients)
- 3.3.1.2. Dative alternation with naar-phrases (goals)
- 3.3.1.3. Dative alternation with van-phrases (sources)
- 3.3.1.4. Dative alternation with bij-phrases (possessors)
- 3.3.1.5. Dative alternation with voor-phrases (benefactives)
- 3.3.1.6. Conclusion
- 3.3.1.7. Bibliographical notes
- 3.3.2. Accusative/PP alternations
- 3.3.3. Nominative/PP alternations
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.4. Some apparent cases of verb frame alternation
- 3.5. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of verb phrases IIIa:Selection of clauses/verb phrases
- 5 Projection of verb phrases IIIb:Argument and complementive clauses
- Introduction
- 5.1. Finite argument clauses
- 5.2. Infinitival argument clauses
- 5.3. Complementive clauses
- 6 Projection of verb phrases IIIc:Complements of non-main verbs
- 7 Projection of verb phrases IIId:Verb clusters
- 8 Projection of verb phrases IV: Adverbial modification
- 9 Word order in the clause I:General introduction
- 10 Word order in the clause II:Position of the finite verb (verb-first/second)
- 11 Word order in the clause III:Clause-initial position (wh-movement)
- Introduction
- 11.1. The formation of V1- and V2-clauses
- 11.2. Clause-initial position remains (phonetically) empty
- 11.3. Clause-initial position is filled
- 12 Word order in the clause IV:Postverbal field (extraposition)
- 13 Word order in the clause V: Middle field (scrambling)
- 14 Main-clause external elements
- Nouns and Noun Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of noun phrases I: complementation
- Introduction
- 2.1. General observations
- 2.2. Prepositional and nominal complements
- 2.3. Clausal complements
- 2.4. Bibliographical notes
- 3 Projection of noun phrases II: modification
- Introduction
- 3.1. Restrictive and non-restrictive modifiers
- 3.2. Premodification
- 3.3. Postmodification
- 3.3.1. Adpositional phrases
- 3.3.2. Relative clauses
- 3.3.3. Infinitival clauses
- 3.3.4. A special case: clauses referring to a proposition
- 3.3.5. Adjectival phrases
- 3.3.6. Adverbial postmodification
- 3.4. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of noun phrases III: binominal constructions
- Introduction
- 4.1. Binominal constructions without a preposition
- 4.2. Binominal constructions with a preposition
- 4.3. Bibliographical notes
- 5 Determiners: articles and pronouns
- Introduction
- 5.1. Articles
- 5.2. Pronouns
- 5.3. Bibliographical notes
- 6 Numerals and quantifiers
- 7 Pre-determiners
- Introduction
- 7.1. The universal quantifier al 'all' and its alternants
- 7.2. The pre-determiner heel 'all/whole'
- 7.3. A note on focus particles
- 7.4. Bibliographical notes
- 8 Syntactic uses of noun phrases
- Adjectives and Adjective Phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- 2 Projection of adjective phrases I: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adjective phrases II: Modification
- 4 Projection of adjective phrases III: Comparison
- 5 Attributive use of the adjective phrase
- 6 Predicative use of the adjective phrase
- 7 The partitive genitive construction
- 8 Adverbial use of the adjective phrase
- 9 Participles and infinitives: their adjectival use
- 10 Special constructions
- Adpositions and adpositional phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Introduction
- 1.1. Characterization of the category adposition
- 1.2. A formal classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3. A semantic classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3.1. Spatial adpositions
- 1.3.2. Temporal adpositions
- 1.3.3. Non-spatial/temporal prepositions
- 1.4. Borderline cases
- 1.5. Bibliographical notes
- 2 Projection of adpositional phrases: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adpositional phrases: Modification
- 4 Syntactic uses of the adpositional phrase
- 5 R-pronominalization and R-words
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Phonology
-
- General
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological Processes
- Assimilation
- Vowel nasalization
- Syllabic sonorants
- Final devoicing
- Fake geminates
- Vowel hiatus resolution
- Vowel reduction introduction
- Schwa deletion
- Schwa insertion
- /r/-deletion
- d-insertion
- {s/z}-insertion
- t-deletion
- Intrusive stop formation
- Breaking
- Vowel shortening
- h-deletion
- Replacement of the glide w
- Word stress
- Clitics
- Allomorphy
- Orthography of Frisian
- Morphology
- Inflection
- Word formation
- Derivation
- Prefixation
- Infixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixes
- Verbal suffixes
- Adjectival suffixes
- Adverbial suffixes
- Numeral suffixes
- Interjectional suffixes
- Onomastic suffixes
- Conversion
- Compositions
- Derivation
- Syntax
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Unergative and unaccusative subjects
- Evidentiality
- To-infinitival clauses
- Predication and noun incorporation
- Ellipsis
- Imperativus-pro-Infinitivo
- Expression of irrealis
- Embedded Verb Second
- Agreement
- Negation
- Nouns & Noun Phrases
- Classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Partitive noun constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Nominalised quantifiers
- Kind partitives
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Bare nominal attributions
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers and (pre)determiners
- Interrogative pronouns
- R-pronouns
- Syntactic uses
- Adjective Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification and degree quantification
- Comparison by degree
- Comparative
- Superlative
- Equative
- Attribution
- Agreement
- Attributive adjectives vs. prenominal elements
- Complex adjectives
- Noun ellipsis
- Co-occurring adjectives
- Predication
- Partitive adjective constructions
- Adverbial use
- Participles and infinitives
- Adposition Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Intransitive adpositions
- Predication
- Preposition stranding
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
-
- General
- Morphology
- Morphology
- 1 Word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 1.1.1 Compounds and their heads
- 1.1.2 Special types of compounds
- 1.1.2.1 Affixoids
- 1.1.2.2 Coordinative compounds
- 1.1.2.3 Synthetic compounds and complex pseudo-participles
- 1.1.2.4 Reduplicative compounds
- 1.1.2.5 Phrase-based compounds
- 1.1.2.6 Elative compounds
- 1.1.2.7 Exocentric compounds
- 1.1.2.8 Linking elements
- 1.1.2.9 Separable Complex Verbs and Particle Verbs
- 1.1.2.10 Noun Incorporation Verbs
- 1.1.2.11 Gapping
- 1.2 Derivation
- 1.3 Minor patterns of word formation
- 1.1 Compounding
- 2 Inflection
- 1 Word formation
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
- 0 Introduction to the AP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of APs
- 2 Complementation of APs
- 3 Modification and degree quantification of APs
- 4 Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative
- 5 Attribution of APs
- 6 Predication of APs
- 7 The partitive adjective construction
- 8 Adverbial use of APs
- 9 Participles and infinitives as APs
- Nouns and Noun Phrases (NPs)
- 0 Introduction to the NP
- 1 Characteristics and Classification of NPs
- 2 Complementation of NPs
- 3 Modification of NPs
- 3.1 Modification of NP by Determiners and APs
- 3.2 Modification of NP by PP
- 3.3 Modification of NP by adverbial clauses
- 3.4 Modification of NP by possessors
- 3.5 Modification of NP by relative clauses
- 3.6 Modification of NP in a cleft construction
- 3.7 Free relative clauses and selected interrogative clauses
- 4 Partitive noun constructions and constructions related to them
- 4.1 The referential partitive construction
- 4.2 The partitive construction of abstract quantity
- 4.3 The numerical partitive construction
- 4.4 The partitive interrogative construction
- 4.5 Adjectival, nominal and nominalised partitive quantifiers
- 4.6 Kind partitives
- 4.7 Partitive predication with a preposition
- 4.8 Bare nominal attribution
- 5 Articles and names
- 6 Pronouns
- 7 Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- 8 Interrogative pronouns
- 9 R-pronouns and the indefinite expletive
- 10 Syntactic functions of Noun Phrases
- Adpositions and Adpositional Phrases (PPs)
- 0 Introduction to the PP
- 1 Characteristics and classification of PPs
- 2 Complementation of PPs
- 3 Modification of PPs
- 4 Bare (intransitive) adpositions
- 5 Predication of PPs
- 6 Form and distribution of adpositions with respect to staticity and construction type
- 7 Adpositional complements and adverbials
- Verbs and Verb Phrases (VPs)
- 0 Introduction to the VP in Saterland Frisian
- 1 Characteristics and classification of verbs
- 2 Unergative and unaccusative subjects and the auxiliary of the perfect
- 3 Evidentiality in relation to perception and epistemicity
- 4 Types of to-infinitival constituents
- 5 Predication
- 5.1 The auxiliary of being and its selection restrictions
- 5.2 The auxiliary of going and its selection restrictions
- 5.3 The auxiliary of continuation and its selection restrictions
- 5.4 The auxiliary of coming and its selection restrictions
- 5.5 Modal auxiliaries and their selection restrictions
- 5.6 Auxiliaries of body posture and aspect and their selection restrictions
- 5.7 Transitive verbs of predication
- 5.8 The auxiliary of doing used as a semantically empty finite auxiliary
- 5.9 Supplementive predication
- 6 The verbal paradigm, irregularity and suppletion
- 7 Verb Second and the word order in main and embedded clauses
- 8 Various aspects of clause structure
- Adjectives and adjective phrases (APs)
-
- General
- Phonology
- Afrikaans phonology
- Segment inventory
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- The diphthongised long vowels /e/, /ø/ and /o/
- The unrounded mid-front vowel /ɛ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /ɑ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /a/
- The rounded mid-high back vowel /ɔ/
- The rounded high back vowel /u/
- The rounded and unrounded high front vowels /i/ and /y/
- The unrounded and rounded central vowels /ə/ and /œ/
- The diphthongs /əi/, /œy/ and /œu/
- Overview of Afrikaans consonants
- The bilabial plosives /p/ and /b/
- The alveolar plosives /t/ and /d/
- The velar plosives /k/ and /g/
- The bilabial nasal /m/
- The alveolar nasal /n/
- The velar nasal /ŋ/
- The trill /r/
- The lateral liquid /l/
- The alveolar fricative /s/
- The velar fricative /x/
- The labiodental fricatives /f/ and /v/
- The approximants /ɦ/, /j/ and /ʋ/
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- Word stress
- The phonetic properties of stress
- Primary stress on monomorphemic words in Afrikaans
- Background to primary stress in monomorphemes in Afrikaans
- Overview of the Main Stress Rule of Afrikaans
- The short vowels of Afrikaans
- Long vowels in monomorphemes
- Primary stress on diphthongs in monomorphemes
- Exceptions
- Stress shifts in place names
- Stress shift towards word-final position
- Stress pattern of reduplications
- Phonological processes
- Vowel related processes
- Consonant related processes
- Homorganic glide insertion
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Phonotactics
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Afrikaans syntax
- Nouns and noun phrases
- Characteristics of the NP
- Classification of nouns
- Complementation of NPs
- Modification of NPs
- Binominal and partitive constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Partitive constructions with nominalised quantifiers
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Binominal name constructions
- Binominal genitive constructions
- Bare nominal attribution
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- Syntactic uses of the noun phrase
- Adjectives and adjective phrases
- Characteristics and classification of the AP
- Complementation of APs
- Modification and Degree Quantification of APs
- Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative degree
- Attribution of APs
- Predication of APs
- The partitive adjective construction
- Adverbial use of APs
- Participles and infinitives as adjectives
- Verbs and verb phrases
- Characterisation and classification
- Argument structure
- Verb frame alternations
- Complements of non-main verbs
- Verb clusters
- Complement clauses
- Adverbial modification
- Word order in the clause: Introduction
- Word order in the clause: position of the finite Verb
- Word order in the clause: Clause-initial position
- Word order in the clause: Extraposition and right-dislocation in the postverbal field
- Word order in the middle field
- Emphatic constructions
- Adpositions and adposition phrases
Chapter 5 and Chapter 6 have discussed cases in which, respectively, a main and a non-main verb take a clause or a smaller verbal projection as their complement. We have seen that finite clauses introduced by the finite complementizer dat'that' or of'whether' and infinitival complement clauses introduced by the complementizer-like element om are normally in extraposed position, that is, placed after the matrix verb in clause-final position. This is illustrated in the examples in (3), in which the matrix verb is underlined and the complement clause is in square brackets with the categorial label CP (= complementizer phrase). Since CPs do not allow splitting, we can put such cases aside for our present purpose.
a. | dat | Jan verwacht [CP | dat | Peter zal | komen]. | |
that | Jan expects | that | Peter will | come | ||
'that Jan expects that Peter will come.' |
b. | dat | Jan betwijfelt [CP | of | Peter zal | komen]. | |
that | Jan doubts | whether | Peter will | come | ||
'that Jan doubts whether Peter will come.' |
c. | dat | Jan | popelt | [CP (om) PRO | te komen]. | |
that | Jan | is.eager | comp | to come | ||
'that Jan is eager to come.' |
Infinitival clauses without the complementizer-like element om are sometimes also in extraposed position, but sometimes also allow or even require clause splitting, which is what we typically find if the complement clause is a bare infinitival clause. This is illustrated in the examples in (4) in which the dependent bare infinitival is in italics; in (4a) the matrix verb is a main verb, whereas in (4b) it is the aspectual non-main verb gaan'to go'. We underlined the verbs in order to push to the fore that clause splitting results in clustering of the verbs in clause-final position.
a. | dat | ik | een liedje | hoorde | zingen. | |
that | I | a song | heard | sing | ||
'that I heard singing a song.' |
b. | dat | Jan | een boek | gaat | lezen. | |
that | Jan | a book | goes | read | ||
'that Jan is going to read a book.' |
The examples in (5) show that in the northern varieties of Dutch, verb clusters are normally impermeable. This is clear from the fact that the objects of the bare infinitives cannot follow the clause-final matrix verbs; see Section 7.4 for a discussion of a number of exceptional cases. The percentage signs indicate, however, that permeation of the cluster is possible in some southern varieties of Dutch, especially in West-Flanders; cf. Sections 5.2.3 and 6.2, and Barbiers (2008:ch.2). For ease of exposition, we will follow the northern intuitions in the discussions below and simply mark permeated verb clusters as unacceptable.
a. | % | dat | ik | hoorde | een liedje | zingen. |
that | I | heard | a song | sing |
b. | % | dat | Jan gaat | een boek | lezen. |
that | Jan goes | a book | read |
Constructions such as (4) are not only special in requiring verb clustering, but also because they exhibit a special behavior in the perfect tense; while verbs governed by a perfect auxiliary normally appear as past participles, the non-finite verbs in (4) appear as infinitives if governed by a perfect auxiliary. This so-called infinitivus-pro-participio (IPP) effect is illustrated in (6).
a. | dat | ik | een liedje | heb | horen/*gehoord | zingen. | |
that | I | a song | have | hear/heard | sing | ||
'that Iʼve heard singing a song.' |
b. | dat | Jan | een boek | is gaan/*gegaan | lezen. | |
that | Jan | a book | is go/gone | read | ||
'that Jan has started to read a book.' |
Section 5.2.2.3 argues that verb clustering and the IPP-effect go hand in hand, and can in fact be used as diagnostic properties of structures exhibiting monoclausal behavior, that is, structures consisting of two separate clauses but behave as if we are dealing with one single clause; see Table 1.
monoclausal | biclausal | |
verb clustering | + | — |
infinitivus-pro-participio | + | — |
If so, the notions of clause splitting and verb clustering do not have the same extension: the extension of the latter is a subset of the extension of the former. That clause splitting need not involve verb clustering in the technical sense of the word can be shown by the examples in (7). Example (7a) first shows that te-infinitival complement clauses may also be split by the finite verb in clause-final position. This example differs from those in (4), however, in that the object of the infinitive may also follow the matrix verb in clause-final position, as is shown by (7b). Example (7a) also differs from those in (4) in that it does not exhibit the IPP-effect; in the perfect-tense example in (7c) the verb beweren'to claim' surfaces in its participial form and cannot surface as an infinitive.
a. | dat | Jan | dat boek | beweert | te lezen. | |
that | Jan | that book | claims | to read | ||
'that Jan is claiming to read that book.' |
b. | dat | Jan | beweertdat boek | te lezen. | |
that | Jan | claims | to read | ||
'that Jan is claiming to read that book.' |
c. | dat | Jan | dat boek | heeft | beweerd/*beweren | te lezen. | |
that | Jan | that book | has | claimed/claim | to read | ||
'that Jan has claimed to read that book.' |
If verb clustering and the IPP-effect do go hand in hand, we have to conclude that (7a) does not involve verb clustering. Section 5.2.2.3 therefore analyzed (7a) as a case of remnant extraposition, that is, the infinitival clause is in extraposed position but its object is extracted from it by leftward movement across the finite verb; see Section 4.4.3, sub IV, for a more extensive introduction to this notion. The structure of this example is therefore as indicated in (8); see Reuland (1981), Den Besten & Rutten (1989), Rutten (1991), Broekhuis et al. (1995), and many others.
dat | Jan dat boeki | beweert [ti | te lezen]. | ||
that | Jan that book | claims | to read |
Support for the analysis in (8) comes from the fact that if the embedded te-infinitive has two (or more) dependents, they may occur on different sides of the matrix verb beweren, although this option is generally considered marked, as compared to the two alternative orders.
a. | dat | Jan | beweert | Marie dat boek | te geven. | |
that | Jan | claims | Marie that book | to give | ||
'that Jan is claiming to give Marie that book.' |
b. | (?) | dat Jan Mariebeweertdat boek te geven. |
c. | dat Jan Mariedat boek beweertte geven. |
Example (9b) can neither be derived by extraposition of the full te-infinitival nor by verb clustering, but it can be derived by what we have called remnant extraposition, that is, extraposition plus leftward movement of the indirect object Marie. If this analysis is viable, example (9c) can, of course, be analyzed in a similar way as (9b) by leftward movement of both the indirect and the direct object. The examples in (9) can thus be analyzed as in (10).
a. | dat Jan beweert [Marie dat boek te geven]. | extraposition |
b. | dat Jan Mariei beweert [ti dat boek te geven]. | remnant extraposition |
c. | dat Jan Mariei dat boekj beweert [titj te geven]. | remnant extraposition |
Section 5.2.2.3 has also shown that some cases of splitting of te-infinitivals do involve verb clustering in the technical sense. Consider the primeless examples in (11), which at first sight suggest that proberen is just like beweren.
a. | dat | Jan dat boek | probeert | te lezen. | |
that | Jan that book | tries | to read | ||
'that Jan is trying to read that book.' |
b. | dat | Jan probeert | dat boek | te lezen. | |
that | Jan tries | that book | to read | ||
'that Jan is trying to read that book.' |
However, when we consider the perfect-tense counterparts of these two examples in (12), we see that they exhibit different behavior with respect to the IPP-effect: whereas the verb proberen can appear either as a participle or as an infinitive in the split pattern, it must appear as a participle in the non-split pattern.
a. | dat | Jan dat boek | heeft | geprobeerd/proberen | te lezen. | |
that | Jan that book | has | tried/try | to read | ||
'that Jan has been trying to read that book.' |
b. | dat | Jan | heeft | geprobeerd/*proberen | dat boek | te lezen. | |
that | Jan | has | tried/try | that book | to read | ||
'that Jan has been trying to read that book.' |
If verb clustering and the IPP-effect are two sides of the same coin, we should conclude that (11a) is actually ambiguous: it involves remnant extraposition if proberen surfaces as a participle in the corresponding perfect-tense construction in (12a), but verb clustering if it surfaces as an infinitive. This conclusion receives more support from a consideration of cases in which the infinitive has two or more dependents. The primeless examples in (13) show that the IPP-effect can only occur if all dependents precede the finite verb in clause-final position.
a. | dat | Jan Mariedat boek | heeft | proberen | te geven. | |
that | Jan Marie that book | has | try | to give | ||
'that Jan has tried to give Marie that book.' |
b. | * | dat | Jan Marie | heeft | proberen | dat boek | te geven. |
that | Jan Marie | has | try | that book | to give |
c. | * | dat | Jan heeft | proberen | Mariedat boek | te geven. |
that | Jan has | try | Marie that book | to give |
The examples in (14) show that all examples in (13) become acceptable if we replace the infinitive proberen by the participle geprobeerd, but then we are no longer dealing with verb clustering but with (remnant) extraposition. As in (9), placing the dependents of the infinitive on different sides of the matrix verb (here: proberen) is generally considered marked, as compared to the alternative orders.
a. | dat | Jan Mariedat boek | heeft | geprobeerd | te geven. | |
that | Jan Marie that book | has | tried | to give |
b. | (?) | dat | Jan Marie | heeft | geprobeerd | dat boek | te geven. |
that | Jan Marie | has | tried | that book | to give |
c. | dat | Jan heeft | geprobeerd | Mariedat boek | te geven. | |
that | Jan has | tried | Marie that book | to give |
The discussion above has revealed that there are two tests which we can apply in order to determine whether we are dealing with verb clustering or (remnant) extraposition: (i) only the former exhibits the IPP-effect, and (ii) only the latter allows permeation of the verbal sequence by the dependents of the embedded main verb (in the northern varieties of Dutch). Our discussion in Sections 7.2 and 7.3 will only consider cases that satisfy both tests.
- 2008Syntactic atlas of the Dutch dialectsAmsterdamAmsterdam University Press
- 1989On verb raising and free word order in DutchJaspers, Dany, Klooster, Wim, Putseys, Yvan & Seuren, Pieter (eds.)Sentential complementation and the lexiconDordrecht/ProvidenceForis Publications41-56
- 1995Infinitival complementation: on remnant extrapositionThe Linguistic Review1293-122
- 1981On extraposition of complement clausesNELS11296-318
- 1991Infinitival complements and auxiliariesAmsterdamUniversity of AmsterdamThesis